Skip to main content

Michael Tomasello CV

James F. Bonk Distinguished Professor
Psychology & Neuroscience
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, 247 Reuben-Cooke Building, Durham, NC 27708
CV

Selected Publications


Three- and 5-year-old children know their current belief might be wrong.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · October 2024 By 4 or 5 years of age, children understand when their own past beliefs were incorrect, or when others' current beliefs are incorrect. In the current study, we asked whether young children understand when their own current belief might be incorrect. 3- and ... Full text Cite

Effects of “We”-framing and partner number on 2- and 3-year-olds’ sense of commitment

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2024 Committed partners feel normatively bound to one another. This normative pressure causes partners not to abandon one another for attractive alternatives. Research suggests that this sense of commitment emerges at around 3 years of age. This study investiga ... Full text Cite

Reciprocal reputation management: Preschoolers respond to shared credit with shared blame

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2024 In a single experiment, we asked whether children would be more likely to accept blame for another's transgression when the individual had previously told a prosocial lie that improved the child's reputation. 3- and 5-year-old children (N=120) were introdu ... Full text Cite

In Response.

Journal Article Anesth Analg · June 6, 2024 Full text Link to item Cite

Gender and cultural differences in the development of reciprocity in young children.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · June 2024 A foundational mechanism underlying human cooperation is reciprocity. In the context of repeated interactions with others, it is not always clear the degree to which in-kind responses reflect responsiveness to partners' prior behaviors ("reactive" response ... Full text Cite

Thought and language: Effects of group-mindedness on young children's interpretation of exclusive we.

Journal Article Child development · May 2024 The current study investigated whether age-related changes in the conceptualization of social groups influences interpretation of the pronoun we. Sixty-four 2- and 4-year-olds (N = 29 female, 50 White-identifying) viewed scenarios in which it was ambiguous ... Full text Cite

From what I want to do to what we decided to do: 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, honor their agreements with peers.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2024 Sometimes we have a personal preference but we agree with others to follow a different course of action. In this study, 3- and 5-year-old children (N = 160) expressed a preference for playing a game one way and were then confronted with peers who expressed ... Full text Cite

Children are eager to take credit for prosocial acts, and cost affects this tendency.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · January 2024 We report two experiments on children's tendency to enhance their reputations through communicative acts. In the experiments, 4-year-olds (N = 120) had the opportunity to inform a social partner that they had helped him in his absence. In a first experimen ... Full text Cite

Effects of group entitativity on young English-speaking children's interpretation of inclusive We.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2024 The pronoun we can be used to refer to various collections of people depending on various pragmatic factors. This article reports the results of two online experiments that investigated children's interpretation of inclusive we, in which the child-listener ... Full text Cite

Instrumental helping motivations of children and chimpanzees

Journal Article Child Development Perspectives · January 1, 2024 Chimpanzees and other great apes seem to be much less cooperative than humans overall, yet they nevertheless reliably help others in many instrumental circumstances. Although in many contexts the helping behavior of chimpanzees is quite similar to that of ... Full text Cite

Differences in the Social Motivations and Emotions of Humans and Other Great Apes.

Journal Article Human nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.) · December 2023 Humans share with other mammals and primates many social motivations and emotions, but they are also much more cooperative than even their closest primate relatives. Here I review recent comparative experiments and analyses that illustrate humans' species- ... Full text Cite

Thought and language: association of groupmindedness with young English-speaking children’s production of pronouns

Journal Article First Language · October 1, 2023 Shared intentionality theory posits that at age 3, children expand their conception of plural agency to include 3- or more-person groups. We sought to determine whether this conceptual shift is detectable in children’s pronoun use. We report the results of ... Full text Cite

A Shared Intentionality Account of Uniquely Human Social Bonding.

Journal Article Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science · October 2023 Many mechanisms of social bonding are common to all primates, but humans seemingly have developed some that are unique to the species. These involve various kinds of interactive experiences-from taking a walk together to having a conversation-whose common ... Full text Cite

Apes reciprocate food positively and negatively.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · May 2023 Reciprocal food exchange is widespread in human societies but not among great apes, who may view food mainly as a target for competition. Understanding the similarities and differences between great apes' and humans' willingness to exchange food is importa ... Full text Cite

Children's consideration of collaboration and merit when making sharing decisions in private.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · April 2023 Young children share equally when they acquire resources through collaboration with a partner, yet it is unclear whether they do so because in such contexts resources are encountered as common and distributed in front of the recipient or because collaborat ... Full text Cite

Just teasing! - Infants' and toddlers' understanding of teasing interactions and its effect on social bonding.

Journal Article Cognition · February 2023 The current study investigates infants' and toddlers' understanding of teasing interactions and its effect on subsequent social interactions. Teasing is a special kind of social interaction due to its dual nature: It consists of a slightly provocative cont ... Full text Cite

Evidence for a developmental shift in the motivation underlying helping in early childhood.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2023 We investigated children's positive emotions as an indicator of their underlying prosocial motivation. In Study 1, 2-, and 5-year-old children (N = 64) could either help an individual or watch as another person provided help. Following the helping event an ... Full text Cite

Social cognition and metacognition in great apes: a theory.

Journal Article Animal cognition · January 2023 Twenty-five years ago, at the founding of this journal, there existed only a few conflicting findings about great apes' social-cognitive skills (theory of mind). In the 2 ½ decades since, we have discovered that great apes understand the goals, intentions, ... Full text Cite

Five-year-old children show cooperative preferences for faces with white sclera.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · January 2023 The cooperative eye hypothesis posits that human eye morphology evolved to facilitate cooperation. Although it is known that young children prefer stimuli with eyes that contain white sclera, it is unknown whether white sclera influences children's percept ... Full text Cite

Having Intentions, Understanding Intentions, and Understanding Communicative Intentions

Chapter · January 1, 2023 This chapter looks at a major cause and a major consequence of the 9-month social-cognitive revolution; and both of these also concern infant intentionality. It argues that young children’s understanding of other persons as intentional agents results in la ... Full text Cite

Disagreement, justification, and equitable moral judgments: A brief training study.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2022 Although theorists agree that social interactions play a major role in moral development, previous research has not experimentally assessed how specific features of social interactions affect children's moral judgments and reasoning. The current study asse ... Full text Cite

Young children judge defection less negatively when there's a good justification

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2022 Morality includes a common ground ranking of values, of which a central theme is that prosocial actions are more justifiable than selfish ones. Learning to distinguish between good versus bad justifications for actions based on a common ground ranking of v ... Full text Cite

How fairness and dominance guide young children's bargaining decisions.

Journal Article Child development · September 2022 Reaching agreements in conflicts is an important developmental challenge. Here, German 5-year-olds (N = 284, 49% female, mostly White, mixed socioeconomic backgrounds; data collection: June 2016-November 2017) faced repeated face-to-face bargaining problem ... Full text Cite

The coordination of attention and action in great apes and humans.

Journal Article Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences · September 2022 Great apes can discern what others are attending to and even direct others' attention to themselves in flexible ways. But they seemingly do not coordinate their attention with one another recursively-understanding that the other is monitoring their attenti ... Full text Cite

What is it like to be a chimpanzee?

Journal Article Synthese · April 1, 2022 Chimpanzees and humans are close evolutionary relatives who behave in many of the same ways based on a similar type of agentive organization. To what degree do they experience the world in similar ways as well? Using contemporary research in evolutionarily ... Full text Cite

Great apes and human children rationally monitor their decisions.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · March 2022 Several species can detect when they are uncertain about what decision to make-revealed by opting out of the choice, or by seeking more information before deciding. However, we do not know whether any nonhuman animals recognize when they need more informat ... Full text Cite

Effects of "we"-framing on young children's commitment, sharing, and helping.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · February 2022 By around 3 years of age, collaboration induces in young children a normative sense of "we" that creates a sense of obligation (e.g., commitment, fairness) toward their collaborative partner. The current study investigated whether this normative sense of w ... Full text Cite

Shared intentionality, reason-giving and the evolution of human culture.

Journal Article Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences · January 2022 The biological approach to culture focuses almost exclusively on processes of social learning, to the neglect of processes of cultural coordination including joint action and shared intentionality. In this paper, we argue that the distinctive features of h ... Full text Cite

Children across societies enforce conventional norms but in culturally variable ways.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · January 2022 Individuals in all societies conform to their cultural group's conventional norms, from how to dress on certain occasions to how to play certain games. It is an open question, however, whether individuals in all societies actively enforce the group's conve ... Full text Cite

THE EARLY ONTOGENY OF HUMAN COOPERATION AND MORALITY

Chapter · January 1, 2022 In this chapter, we consider children’s moral development from an evolutionary perspective. We propose that human morality arose evolutionarily as a set of skills and motives for cooperating with others. Following recent accounts by Tomasello and colleague ... Full text Cite

Knowledge-by-acquaintance before propositional knowledge/belief.

Journal Article The Behavioral and brain sciences · November 2021 More basic than the authors' distinction between knowing and believing is a distinction between knowledge-by-acquaintance (I know John Smith) and propositional knowledge/belief (I know/believe that John Smith lives in Durham). This distinction provides a b ... Full text Cite

Who can be in a group? 3- to 5-year-old children construe realistic social groups through mutual intentionality

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2021 Recent research suggests that young children's causal justification for minimal group membership can be induced via a cognitive framework of mutual intentionality. That is, an individual can become a group member when both the individual and group agree to ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of justifications for breaking a promise

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2021 There are sometimes legitimate reasons for breaking a promise when circumstances change. We investigated 3- and 5-year-old German children's understanding of promise breaking in prosocial (helping someone else) and selfish (playing with someone else) condi ... Full text Cite

Toddlers Prefer Adults as Informants: 2- and 3-Year-Olds' Use of and Attention to Pointing Gestures From Peer and Adult Partners.

Journal Article Child development · July 2021 Two- and 3-year-old children (N = 96) were tested in an object-choice task with video presentations of peer and adult partners. An immersive, semi-interactive procedure enabled both the close matching of adult and peer conditions and the combination of par ... Full text Cite

Response to: Rethinking Human Development and the Shared Intentionality Hypothesis

Journal Article Review of Philosophy and Psychology · June 1, 2021 I respond to Moll, Nichols, and Mackey’s review of my book Becoming Human. I agree with many of their points, but have my own point of view on some others. ... Full text Cite

Norms Require Not Just Technical Skill and Social Learning, but Real Cooperation

Journal Article Analyse und Kritik · June 1, 2021 Birch's account of the evolutionary origins of social norms is essentially individualistic. It begins with individuals regulating their own actions toward internally represented goals, as evaluative standards, and adds in a social dimension only secondaril ... Full text Cite

The Development of the Liking Gap: Children Older Than 5 Years Think That Partners Evaluate Them Less Positively Than They Evaluate Their Partners.

Journal Article Psychological science · May 2021 After two strangers have briefly interacted with one another, both believe that they like their partner more than their partner likes them. A plausible explanation for this liking gap is that people are constantly worrying about how others are evalu ... Full text Cite

Joint attention to mental content and the social origin of reasoning

Journal Article Synthese · May 1, 2021 Growing evidence indicates that our higher rational capacities depend on social interaction—that only through engaging with others do we acquire the ability to evaluate beliefs as true or false, or to reflect on and evaluate the reasons that support our be ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) internal arousal remains elevated if they cannot themselves help a conspecific.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · May 2021 Chimpanzees help conspecifics achieve their goals in instrumental situations, but neither their immediate motivation nor the evolutionary basis of their motivation is clear. In the current study, we gave chimpanzees the opportunity to instrumentally help a ... Full text Cite

Common knowledge that help is needed increases helping behavior in children.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · January 2021 Although there is considerable evidence that at least some helping behavior is motivated by genuine concern for others' well-being, sometimes we also help solely out of a sense of obligation to the persons in need. Our sense of obligation to help may be pa ... Full text Cite

Young children's moral judgments depend on the social relationship between agents

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2021 Moral judgments can vary depending on the social relationship between agents. We presented 4- and 6-year-old peer dyads (N = 128) with stories, in which a parent (parent condition) or a peer protagonist (peer condition) faced a child in need of help (e.g., ... Full text Cite

Collaborative reasoning in the context of group competition.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2021 A key skill in collaborative problem-solving is to communicate and evaluate reasons for proposals to arrive at the decision benefiting all group members. Although it is well-documented that collaborative contexts facilitate young children's reasoning, less ... Full text Cite

On the moral functions of language

Journal Article Social Cognition · 2021 Cite

Young children share more under time pressure than after a delay.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2021 Adults under time pressure share with others generously, but with more time they act more selfishly. In the current study, we investigated whether young children already operate in this same way, and, if so, whether this changes over the preschool and earl ... Full text Cite

Young children conform more to norms than to preferences.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2021 As members of cultural groups, humans continually adhere to social norms and conventions. Researchers have hypothesized that even young children are motivated to act conventionally, but support for this hypothesis has been indirect and open to other interp ... Full text Open Access Cite

Fathers, siblings, and the bridge hypothesis

Chapter · January 1, 2021 The underlying assumption of the Bridge Hypothesis-which is not shared by all developmental psycholinguists-is that communication pressure is beneficial to the child’s development of communicative competence, including the acquisition of linguistic skills. ... Full text Cite

The Early Ontogeny of Reason Giving

Journal Article Child Development Perspectives · December 1, 2020 The key context within which preschool children learn to justify beliefs with reasons is collaborative problem-solving and decision-making with peers, including in the moral domain, in which they engage with another coequal mind in a cooperative spirit. Ev ... Full text Cite

Human children, but not great apes, become socially closer by sharing an experience in common ground.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2020 To create social closeness, humans engage in a variety of social activities centered around shared experiences. Even simply watching the same video side by side creates social closeness in adults and children. However, perhaps surprisingly, a similar psych ... Full text Cite

The development of intent-based moral judgment and moral behavior in the context of indirect reciprocity: A cross-cultural study

Journal Article International Journal of Behavioral Development · November 1, 2020 The current study aimed to investigate the cultural differences in the developmental origins of children’s intent-based moral judgment and moral behavior in the context of indirect reciprocity. To this end, we compared how German and Chinese children inter ... Full text Cite

Do 7-year-old children understand social leverage?

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2020 Individuals with an advantageous position during a negotiation possess leverage over their partners. Several studies with adults have investigated how leverage can influence the coordination strategies of individuals when conflicts of interest arise. In th ... Full text Cite

Young children's prosocial responses toward peers and adults in two social contexts.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · October 2020 Young children help and share with others, but little is known about the "how" and "who" of this early prosocial behavior. In the current study, we compared 2- and 3-year-old children's (N = 203; 101 girls) prosocial behavior of sharing and helping. We ask ... Full text Cite

Young children show positive emotions when seeing someone get the help they deserve

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2020 Little is known about the underlying emotional bases of children's prosociality. Here we engaged 32 dyads of 4-year-old children in a reward-collecting task at the end of which one child was more in need of help. An adult then either helped the needier chi ... Full text Cite

The Ontogenetic Foundations of Epistemic Norms

Journal Article Episteme · September 1, 2020 In this paper, I approach epistemic norms from an ontogenetic point of view. I argue and present evidence that to understand epistemic norms - e.g., scientific norms of methodology and the evaluation of evidence - children must first develop through their ... Full text Cite

Why don't apes point?

Chapter · August 22, 2020 Full text Cite

The gestural communication of apes and monkeys

Book · August 19, 2020 The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys is an intriguing compilation of naturalistic and experimental research conducted over the course of 20 years on gestural communication in primates, as well as a comparison to what is known about the vocal comm ... Full text Cite

Learning Novel Skills From Iconic Gestures: A Developmental and Evolutionary Perspective.

Journal Article Psychological science · July 2020 Cumulative cultural learning has been argued to rely on high-fidelity copying of other individuals' actions. Iconic gestures of actions have no physical effect on objects in the world but merely represent actions that would have an effect. Learning from ic ... Full text Cite

Introduction to special issue: 'Life history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and culture in humans and other animals'.

Journal Article Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences · July 2020 This special issue focuses on the relationship between life history and learning, especially during human evolution. 'Life history' refers to the developmental programme of an organism, including its period of immaturity, reproductive rate and timing, care ... Full text Cite

The adaptive origins of uniquely human sociality.

Journal Article Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences · July 2020 Humans possess some unique social-cognitive skills and motivations, involving such things as joint attention, cooperative communication, dual-level collaboration and cultural learning. These are almost certainly adaptations for humans' especially complex s ... Full text Cite

The development of coordination via joint expectations for shared benefits.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · June 2020 People frequently need to cooperate despite having strong self-serving motives. In the current study, pairs of 5- and 7-year-olds (N = 160) faced a one-shot coordination problem: To benefit, children had to choose the same of 3 reward divisions. The ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees help others with what they want; children help them with what they need.

Journal Article Developmental science · May 2020 Humans, including young children, are strongly motivated to help others, even paying a cost to do so. Humans' nearest primate relatives, great apes, are likewise motivated to help others, raising the question of whether the motivations of humans and apes a ... Full text Cite

Young Children's Ability to Produce Valid and Relevant Counter-Arguments.

Journal Article Child development · May 2020 In collaborative problem solving, children produce and evaluate arguments for proposals. We investigated whether 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 192) can produce and evaluate arguments against those arguments (i.e., counter-arguments). In Study 1, each child withi ... Full text Cite

Preschoolers refer to direct and indirect evidence in their collaborative reasoning.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · May 2020 Collaborative reasoning requires partners to evaluate options and the evidence for or against each option. We investigated whether preschoolers can explain why one option is best (direct reasons) and why the other option is not (indirect reasons), looking ... Full text Cite

The psychological mechanisms underlying reciprocal prosociality in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · May 2020 In both the wild and captivity, chimpanzees engage in reciprocal patterns of prosocial behavior. However, the proximate mechanisms underlying these patterns are unclear. In the current study, we investigated whether chimpanzees prefer to act prosocially to ... Full text Cite

The strategies used by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) to solve a simple coordination problem.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · May 2020 One of the challenges of collaboration is to coordinate decisions with others, and recent theories have proposed that humans, in particular, evolved skills to address this challenge. To test this hypothesis, we compared the coordination abilities of 4-year ... Full text Cite

The many faces of obligation.

Journal Article The Behavioral and brain sciences · April 2020 My response to the commentaries focuses on four issues: (1) the diversity both within and between cultures of the many different faces of obligation; (2) the possible evolutionary roots of the sense of obligation, including possible sources that I did not ... Full text Cite

The role of roles in uniquely human cognition and sociality

Journal Article Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour · March 1, 2020 To understand themselves as playing a social role, individuals must understand themselves to be contributing to a cooperative endeavor. Psychologically, the form of cooperation required is a specific type that only humans may possess, namely, one in which ... Full text Cite

Children, but not great apes, respect ownership.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2020 Access to and control of resources is a major source of costly conflicts. Animals, under some conditions, respect what others control and use (i.e. possession). Humans not only respect possession of resources, they also respect ownership. Ownership can be ... Full text Cite

Watching a video together creates social closeness between children and adults.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · January 2020 Human social relationships are often formed through shared social activities in which individuals share mental states about external stimuli. Previous work on joint attention has shown that even minimal shared experiences such as watching something togethe ... Full text Cite

Early birds: Metaphor understanding in 3-year-olds

Journal Article Journal of Pragmatics · January 1, 2020 To assess children's cognitive capacities to understand (rather than explain or paraphrase) metaphors, we investigated how 3-year-olds (n = 36; 3;0–3;3) fare with novel metaphors corresponding to their world knowledge and linguistic competences using a beh ... Full text Cite

Foreword

Book · January 1, 2020 Full text Cite

Toddlers' intrinsic motivation to return help to their benefactor.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · December 2019 A natural reaction to receiving help from someone is to help that person in return. In two studies, we investigated the developmental origins of children's motivation to return help. In Study 1, 18- and 24-month-old toddlers were either helped or not helpe ... Full text Cite

Young children spontaneously recreate core properties of language in a new modality.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · December 2019 How the world's 6,000+ natural languages have arisen is mostly unknown. Yet, new sign languages have emerged recently among deaf people brought together in a community, offering insights into the dynamics of language evolution. However, documenting the eme ... Full text Cite

Children's Selective Trust in Promises.

Journal Article Child development · November 2019 There has been extensive research into the development of selective trust in testimony, but little is known about the development of selective trust in promises. The present research investigates children's (N = 264) selective trust in others' promises to ... Full text Cite

The influence of intention and outcome on young children's reciprocal sharing.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2019 This study investigated the influence of underlying intentions and outcomes of a partner's sharing behavior on young children's reciprocity. We provided 3- and 5-year-old children with the opportunity to share with a partner following different treatments ... Full text Cite

Adult instruction limits children's flexibility in moral decision making.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2019 Children's moral behavior is guided, in part, by adults teaching children how to treat others. However, when circumstances change, such instructions may become either unhelpful or limiting. In the current study, 48 dyads of 5-year-olds played a collaborati ... Full text Cite

Children's respect for ownership across diverse societies.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2019 Ownership is a cornerstone of many human societies and can be understood as a cooperative arrangement, where individuals refrain from taking each other's property. Owners can thus trust others to respect their property even in their absence. We investigate ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees use observed temporal directionality to learn novel causal relations.

Journal Article Primates; journal of primatology · November 2019 We investigated whether chimpanzees use the temporal sequence of external events to determine causation. Seventeen chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) witnessed a human experimenter press a button in two different conditions. When she pressed the "causal button" ... Full text Cite

Children's reasoning with peers and parents about moral dilemmas.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2019 Children encounter moral norms in several different social contexts. Often it is in hierarchically structured interactions with parents or other adults, but sometimes it is in more symmetrically structured interactions with peers. Our question was whether ... Full text Cite

Children choose to reason with partners who submit to reason

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2019 When reasoning with others, the reasons used in an exchange can have varying degrees of quality, irrespective of the facts under discussion. Partners often evaluate one another's evaluation of reasons – one another's reasoning. Can children evaluate their ... Full text Cite

The Origins of Morality

Journal Article Scientific American · September 2019 Full text Cite

Respect Defended.

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · September 2019 Full text Cite

Three- and 5-year-old children's understanding of how to dissolve a joint commitment.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · August 2019 When young children form a joint commitment with a partner, they understand that this agreement generates obligations. In this study, we investigated whether young children understand that joint commitments, and their associated obligations, may likewise b ... Full text Open Access Cite

Thirty years of great ape gestures.

Journal Article Animal cognition · July 2019 We and our colleagues have been doing studies of great ape gestural communication for more than 30 years. Here we attempt to spell out what we have learned. Some aspects of the process have been reliably established by multiple researchers, for example, it ... Full text Open Access Cite

Eighteen-Month-Old Infants Correct Non-Conforming Actions by Others.

Journal Article Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies · July 2019 At around their third birthday, children begin to enforce social norms on others impersonally, often using generic normative language, but little is known about the developmental building blocks of this abstract norm understanding. Here, we investigate whe ... Full text Cite

Visually attending to a video together facilitates great ape social closeness.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · July 2019 Humans create social closeness with one another through a variety of shared social activities in which they align their emotions or mental states towards an external stimulus such as dancing to music together, playing board games together or even engaging ... Full text Cite

Children's Sense of Fairness as Equal Respect.

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · June 2019 One influential view holds that children's sense of fairness emerges at age 8 and is rooted in the development of an aversion to unequal resource distributions. Here, we suggest two amendments to this view. First, we argue and present evidence that childre ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees monopolize and children take turns in a limited resource problem.

Journal Article Scientific reports · May 2019 Competition over scarce resources is common across the animal kingdom. Here we investigate the strategies of chimpanzees and children in a limited resource problem. Both species were presented with a tug-of-war apparatus in which each individual in a dyad ... Full text Cite

The moral psychology of obligation.

Journal Article The Behavioral and brain sciences · May 2019 Although psychologists have paid scant attention to the sense of obligation as a distinctly human motivation, moral philosophers have identified two of its key features: First, it has a peremptory, demanding force, with a kind of coercive quality, and seco ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) coordinate by communicating in a collaborative problem-solving task.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · April 2019 Successful collaboration often relies on individuals' capacity to communicate with each other. Despite extensive research on chimpanzee communication, there is little evidence that chimpanzees are capable, without extensive human training, of regulating co ... Full text Cite

The relation between young children's physiological arousal and their motivation to help others.

Journal Article Neuropsychologia · March 2019 Children are motivated to help others from an early age. However, little is known about the internal biological mechanisms underlying their motivation to help. Here, we compiled data from five separate studies in which children, ranging in age from 18 mont ... Full text Open Access Cite

Natural reference: A phylo- and ontogenetic perspective on the comprehension of iconic gestures and vocalizations.

Journal Article Developmental science · March 2019 The recognition of iconic correspondence between signal and referent has been argued to bootstrap the acquisition and emergence of language. Here, we study the ontogeny, and to some extent the phylogeny, of the ability to spontaneously relate iconic signal ... Full text Cite

Children engage in competitive altruism.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2019 Humans cultivate their reputations as good cooperators, sometimes even competing with group mates, to appear most cooperative to individuals during the process of selecting partners. To investigate the ontogenetic origins of such "competitive altruism," we ... Full text Cite

Children use rules to coordinate in a social dilemma.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2019 Humans are frequently required to coordinate their actions in social dilemmas (e.g. when one of two drivers has to yield for the other at an intersection). This is commonly achieved by individuals following communally known rules that prescribe how people ... Full text Cite

Young children's reputational strategies in a peer group context.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · February 2019 Reputational concerns are known to promote cooperation. Individuals regularly act more prosocially when their behavior is observable by others. Here, we investigate 4- and 5-year-old (N = 144) children's reputational strategies in a competitive group setti ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees and children avoid mutual defection in a social dilemma

Journal Article Evolution and Human Behavior · January 1, 2019 Cooperation often comes with the temptation to defect and benefit at the cost of others. This tension between cooperation and defection is best captured in social dilemmas like the Prisoner's Dilemma. Adult humans have specific strategies to maintain coope ... Full text Cite

3- and 5-year-old children's adherence to explicit and implicit joint commitments.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · January 2019 The problem with collaboration is that there are temptations to defect. Explicit joint commitments are designed to mitigate some of the risks, but people also feel committed to others implicitly when they both know together that they each hold the other's ... Full text Cite

Preschoolers consider (absent) others when choosing a distribution procedure.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2019 This study investigated how the presence of others and anticipated distributions for self influence children's fairness-related decisions in two different socio-moral contexts. In the first part, three- and five-year-old children (N = 120) decided between ... Full text Cite

How chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) share the spoils with collaborators and bystanders.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2019 Chimpanzees hunt cooperatively in the wild, but the factors influencing food sharing after the hunt are not well understood. In an experimental study, groups of three captive chimpanzees obtained a monopolizable food resource, either via two individuals co ... Full text Cite

Punishment

Chapter · January 1, 2019 Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be motivated by its effec ... Full text Cite

Punishment

Chapter · January 1, 2019 Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be motivated by its effec ... Full text Cite

Communicative eye contact signals a commitment to cooperate for young children.

Journal Article Cognition · October 2018 Making commitments to cooperate facilitates cooperation. There is a long-standing theoretical debate about how promissory obligations come into existence, and whether linguistic acts (such as saying "I promise") are a necessary part of the process. To info ... Full text Cite

Why should I trust you? Investigating young children's spontaneous mistrust in potential deceivers

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2018 Children must learn not to trust everyone to avoid being taken advantage of. In the current study, 5- and 7-year-old children were paired with a partner whose incentives were either congruent (cooperative condition) or conflicting (competitive condition) w ... Full text Cite

The normative turn in early moral development

Journal Article Human Development · October 1, 2018 The Cooperation Theory of moral development starts from the premise that morality is a special form of cooperation. Before 3 years of age, children help and share with others prosocially, and they collaborate with others in ways that foster a sense of equa ... Full text Cite

Three-Year-Olds' Reactions to a Partner's Failure to Perform Her Role in a Joint Commitment.

Journal Article Child development · September 2018 When children make a joint commitment to collaborate, obligations are created. Pairs of 3-year-old children (N = 144) made a joint commitment to play a game. In three different conditions the game was interrupted in the middle either because: (a) the partn ... Full text Open Access Cite

Great Apes and Human Development: A Personal History

Journal Article Child Development Perspectives · September 1, 2018 In this article, I recount my history of research with great apes. From the beginning, the idea was to compare apes to human children, with an eye to discovering facts relevant to describing and explaining processes of human development. The research went ... Full text Open Access Cite

Two-year-olds use adults' but not peers' points.

Journal Article Developmental science · September 2018 In the current study, 24- to 27-month-old children (N = 37) used pointing gestures in a cooperative object choice task with either peer or adult partners. When indicating the location of a hidden toy, children pointed equally accurately for adult and peer ... Full text Cite

The social-cognitive basis of infants' reference to absent entities.

Journal Article Cognition · August 2018 Recent evidence suggests that infants as young as 12 month of age use pointing to communicate about absent entities. The tacit assumption underlying these studies is that infants do so based on tracking what their interlocutor experienced in a previous sha ... Full text Cite

The preference for scarcity: A developmental and comparative perspective

Journal Article Psychology and Marketing · August 1, 2018 Human adults often show a preference for scarce over abundant goods. In this paper, we investigate whether this preference was shared by 4- and 6-year-old children as well as chimpanzees, humans’ nearest primate relative. Neither chimpanzees nor 4-year-old ... Full text Cite

How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · August 2018 To predict and explain the behavior of others, one must understand that their actions are determined not by reality but by their beliefs about reality. Classically, children come to understand beliefs, including false beliefs, at about 4-5 y of age, but re ... Full text Cite

Precís of a natural history of human morality

Journal Article Philosophical Psychology · July 4, 2018 Here I summarize the main points in my 2016 book, A Natural History of Human Morality. Taking an evolutionary point of view, I characterize human morality as a special form of cooperation. In particular, human morality represents a kind of we > me orientat ... Full text Cite

Response to commentators

Journal Article Philosophical Psychology · July 4, 2018 Full text Cite

Modeling social norms increasingly influences costly sharing in middle childhood.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · July 2018 Prosocial and normative behavior emerges in early childhood, but substantial changes in prosocial behavior in middle childhood may be due to it becoming integrated with children's understanding of what is normative. Here we show that information about what ... Full text Cite

Children's reasoning with peers in cooperative and competitive contexts.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · March 2018 We report two studies that demonstrate how five- and seven-year-olds adapt their production of arguments to either a cooperative or a competitive context. Two games elicited agreements from peer dyads about placing animals on either of two halves of a play ... Full text Open Access Cite

The specificity of reciprocity: Young children reciprocate more generously to those who intentionally benefit them.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2018 Young children engage in direct reciprocity, but the mechanisms underlying such reciprocity remain unclear. In particular, prior work leaves unclear whether children's reciprocity is simply a response to receiving benefits (regardless of whether the benefi ... Full text Open Access Cite

The development of intention-based sociomoral judgment and distribution behavior from a third-party stance.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2018 The current study investigated children's intention-based sociomoral judgments and distribution behavior from a third-party stance. An actor puppet showed either positive or negative intention toward a target puppet, which had previously performed a prosoc ... Full text Cite

Young children are more willing to accept group decisions in which they have had a voice.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · February 2018 People accept an unequal distribution of resources if they judge that the decision-making process was fair. In this study, 3- and 5-year-old children played an allocation game with two puppets. The puppets decided against a fair distribution in all conditi ... Full text Open Access Cite

Children's meta-talk in their collaborative decision making with peers.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · February 2018 In collaborative decision making, children must evaluate the evidence behind their respective claims and the rationality of their respective proposals with their partners. In the main study, 5- and 7-year-old peer dyads (N = 196) were presented with a nove ... Full text Open Access Cite

Concern for Group Reputation Increases Prosociality in Young Children.

Journal Article Psychological science · February 2018 The motivation to build and maintain a positive personal reputation promotes prosocial behavior. But individuals also identify with their groups, and so it is possible that the desire to maintain or enhance group reputation may have similar effects. Here, ... Full text Open Access Cite

The reasons young children give to peers when explaining their judgments of moral and conventional rules.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · February 2018 Moral justifications work, when they do, by invoking values that are shared in the common ground of the interlocutors. We asked 3- and 5-year-old peer dyads (N = 144) to identify and punish norm transgressors. In the moral condition, the transgressor viola ... Full text Open Access Cite

The goal of ape pointing.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2018 Captive great apes regularly use pointing gestures in their interactions with humans. However, the precise function of this gesture is unknown. One possibility is that apes use pointing primarily to direct attention (as in "please look at that"); another i ... Full text Cite

Constructively combining languages

Journal Article Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism · January 1, 2018 Language development in bilingual children is often related to differing levels of proficiency. Objective measurements of bilingual development include for example mean length of utterance (MLU). MLU is almost always calculated for each language context (i ... Full text Cite

What did we learn from theape language studies?

Chapter · January 1, 2018 The ‘ape language’ studies have come and gone, with wildly divergent claims about what they have shown. Without question, the most sophisticated skills have been displayed by Kanzi, a male bonobo exposed from youth to a human-like communicative system. Thi ... Full text Cite

Identifying partially schematic units in the code-mixing of an English and German speaking child

Journal Article Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism · January 1, 2018 Intra-sentential code-mixing presents a number of puzzles for theories of bilingualism. In this paper, we examine the code-mixed English-German utterances of a young English-German-Spanish trilingual child between 1;10 – 3;1, using both an extensive diary ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees' understanding of social leverage.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2018 Social primates can influence others through the control of resources. For instance, dominant male chimpanzees might allow subordinates access to mate with females in exchange for social support. However, little is known about how chimpanzees strategically ... Full text Cite

Children's developing metaethical judgments.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · December 2017 Human adults incline toward moral objectivism but may approach things more relativistically if different cultures are involved. In this study, 4-, 6-, and 9-year-old children (N=136) witnessed two parties who disagreed about moral matters: a normative judg ... Full text Open Access Cite

Toddlers Help a Peer.

Journal Article Child development · September 2017 Toddlers are remarkably prosocial toward adults, yet little is known about their helping behavior toward peers. In the present study with 18- and 30-month-old toddlers (n = 192, 48 dyads per age group), one child needed help reaching an object to continue ... Full text Open Access Cite

The Role of Ontogeny in the Evolution of Human Cooperation.

Journal Article Human nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.) · September 2017 To explain the evolutionary emergence of uniquely human skills and motivations for cooperation, Tomasello et al. (2012, in Current Anthropology 53(6):673-92) proposed the interdependence hypothesis. The key adaptive context in this account was the obligate ... Full text Open Access Cite

Submentalizing Cannot Explain Belief-Based Action Anticipation in Apes.

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · September 2017 Full text Open Access Cite

Do young children preferentially trust gossip or firsthand observation in choosing a collaborative partner?

Journal Article Social Development · August 1, 2017 From early on in ontogeny, young children hear things being said about particular individuals. Here we investigate the ways in which testimony with social content, that is, gossip, influences children's decision-making. We explored whether five-year-old (N ... Full text Open Access Cite

Social disappointment explains chimpanzees' behaviour in the inequity aversion task.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · August 2017 Chimpanzees' refusal of less-preferred food when an experimenter has previously provided preferred food to a conspecific has been taken as evidence for a sense of fairness. Here, we present a novel hypothesis-the social disappointment hypothesis-according ... Full text Open Access Cite

Children, chimpanzees, and bonobos adjust the visibility of their actions for cooperators and competitors.

Journal Article Scientific reports · August 2017 Chimpanzees and bonobos are highly capable of tracking other's mental states. It has been proposed, however, that in contrast to humans, chimpanzees are only able to do this in competitive interactions but this has rarely been directly tested. Here, pairs ... Full text Open Access Cite

Children's Intrinsic Motivation to Provide Help Themselves After Accidentally Harming Others.

Journal Article Child development · July 2017 Little is known about the flexibility of children's prosocial motivation. Here, 2- and 3-year-old children's (n = 128) internal arousal, as measured via changes in pupil dilation, was increased after they accidentally harmed a victim but were unable to rep ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young children mostly keep, and expect others to keep, their promises.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · July 2017 Promises are speech acts that create an obligation to do the promised action. In three studies, we investigated whether 3- and 5-year-olds (N=278) understand the normative implications of promising in prosocial interactions. In Study 1, children helped a p ... Full text Open Access Cite

Chimpanzees return favors at a personal cost.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · July 2017 Humans regularly provide others with resources at a personal cost to themselves. Chimpanzees engage in some cooperative behaviors in the wild as well, but their motivational underpinnings are unclear. In three experiments, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes Full text Open Access Cite

Chimpanzees, bonobos, and children successfully coordinate in conflict situations.

Journal Article Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences · June 7, 2017 Social animals need to coordinate with others to reap the benefits of group-living even when individuals’ interests are misaligned. We compare how chimpanzees, bonobos and children coordinate their actions with a conspecific in a Snowdrift game, which prov ... Full text Open Access Cite

From imitation to implementation: How two- and three-year-old children learn to enforce social norms.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · June 2017 Young children enforce social norms from early on, but little research has examined how this enforcement behaviour emerges. This study investigated whether observing an adult's norm enforcement influences children's own enforcement of that norm compared wi ... Full text Open Access Cite

The impact of choice on young children's prosocial motivation.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · June 2017 The current study explored how freedom of choice affects preschoolers' prosocial motivation. Children (3- and 5-year-olds) participated in either a choice condition (where they could decide for themselves whether to help or not) or a no-choice condition (w ... Full text Open Access Cite

Children’s Developing Understanding of the Conventionality of Rules

Journal Article Journal of Cognition and Development · March 15, 2017 Much research has investigated how children relate to norms taught to them by adult authorities. Very few studies have investigated norms that arise out of children’s own peer interactions. In two studies, we investigated how 5- and 7-year-old children tea ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young children, but not chimpanzees, are averse to disadvantageous and advantageous inequities.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2017 The age at which young children show an aversion to inequitable resource distributions, especially those favoring themselves, is unclear. It is also unclear whether great apes, as humans' nearest evolutionary relatives, have an aversion to inequitable reso ... Full text Open Access Cite

Children coordinate in a recurrent social dilemma by taking turns and along dominance asymmetries.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · February 2017 Humans constantly have to coordinate their decisions with others even when their interests are conflicting (e.g., when 2 drivers have to decide who yields at an intersection). So far, however, little is known about the development of these abilities. Here, ... Full text Open Access Cite

The fulfillment of others' needs elevates children's body posture.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · January 2017 Much is known about young children's helping behavior, but little is known about the underlying motivations and emotions involved. In 2 studies we found that 2-year-old children showed positive emotions of similar magnitude-as measured by changes in their ... Full text Open Access Cite

Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2017 Understanding the behavior of others in a wide variety of circumstances requires an understanding of their psychological states. Humans' nearest primate relatives, the great apes, understand many psychological states of others, for example, perceptions, go ... Full text Open Access Cite

A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes' performance in a false belief task inanimate control.

Journal Article Communicative & integrative biology · January 2017 Much debate concerns whether any nonhuman animals share with humans the ability to infer others' mental states, such as desires and beliefs. In a recent eye-tracking false-belief task, we showed that great apes correctly anticipated that a human actor woul ... Full text Open Access Cite

Prosociality and morality in children and chimpanzees

Chapter · January 1, 2017 The modern study of moral development began with Piaget’s (1932) The Moral Judgment of the Child, which although originally published in the 1930s only became internationally known in the 1960s through its influence on Kohlberg’s theory (e.g. Kohlberg, 198 ... Full text Cite

The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure

Book · January 1, 2017 This book, which gathers in one place the theories of 10 leading cognitive and functional linguists, represents a new approach that may define the next era in the history of psychology: It promises to give psychologists a new appreciation of what this vari ... Full text Cite

Great apes are sensitive to prior reliability of an informant in a gaze following task.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2017 Social animals frequently rely on information from other individuals. This can be costly in case the other individual is mistaken or even deceptive. Human infants below 4 years of age show proficiency in their reliance on differently reliable informants. T ... Full text Cite

Toddlers Help Anonymously

Journal Article Infancy · January 1, 2017 Young children are extremely motivated to help others, but it is not clear whether they do so in anonymous situations without social recognition. In two studies, we found that 18-month-old toddlers provided help equally in situations where an adult recipie ... Full text Cite

The Middle Step: Joint Intentionality as a Human-Unique Form of Second-Personal Engagement

Chapter · January 1, 2017 The crucial middle step consists in second-personal engagement with engagement others. In this chapter, the authors present face-to-face interactions joint intentionality. On the other hand there are capacities for acting collectively within a social group ... Full text Cite

How to Compare Across Species.

Journal Article Psychological science · December 2016 Full text Cite

How chimpanzees cooperate: If dominance is artificially constrained.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · November 2016 Full text Open Access Cite

The Early Emergence of Guilt-Motivated Prosocial Behavior.

Journal Article Child development · November 2016 Guilt serves vital prosocial functions: It motivates transgressors to make amends, thus restoring damaged relationships. Previous developmental research on guilt has not clearly distinguished it from sympathy for a victim or a tendency to repair damage in ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young Children Want to See Others Get the Help They Need.

Journal Article Child development · November 2016 Children's instrumental helping has sometimes been interpreted as a desire to complete action sequences or to restore the physical order of things. Two-year-old children (n = 51) selectively retrieved for an adult the object he needed rather than one he di ... Full text Open Access Cite

The role of past interactions in great apes' communication about absent entities.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · November 2016 Recent evidence suggests that great apes can use the former location of an entity to communicate about it. In this study we built on these findings to investigate the social-cognitive foundations of great apes' communicative abilities. We tested whether gr ... Full text Cite

Children's understanding of first- and third-person perspectives in complement clauses and false-belief tasks.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2016 De Villiers (Lingua, 2007, Vol. 117, pp. 1858-1878) and others have claimed that children come to understand false belief as they acquire linguistic constructions for representing a proposition and the speaker's epistemic attitude toward that proposition. ... Full text Cite

Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · October 2016 Featured Publication Humans operate with a "theory of mind" with which they are able to understand that others' actions are driven not by reality but by beliefs about reality, even when those beliefs are false. Although great apes share with humans many social-cognitive skills ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young Children See a Single Action and Infer a Social Norm.

Journal Article Psychological science · October 2016 Featured Publication Human social life depends heavily on social norms that prescribe and proscribe specific actions. Typically, young children learn social norms from adult instruction. In the work reported here, we showed that this is not the whole story: Three-year-old chil ... Full text Open Access Cite

Language in a New Key.

Journal Article Scientific American · October 2016 Full text Cite

Do young children accept responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members?

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2016 This study investigated whether young children accept responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members. Five-year-old children watched a transgressor break someone else's valued possession. Depending on condition, this transgressor either belonge ... Full text Cite

Young children's behavioral and emotional responses to different social norm violations.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · October 2016 From an early age, children can talk meaningfully about differences between moral and conventional norms. But does their understanding of these differences manifest itself in their actual behavioral and emotional reactions to norm violations? And do childr ... Full text Cite

Jerome Seymour Bruner [1915-2016].

Journal Article Journal of child language · September 2016 Full text Open Access Cite

Preschoolers affect others' reputations through prosocial gossip.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · September 2016 Providing evaluative information to others about absent third parties helps them to identify cooperators and avoid cheaters. Here, we show that 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, reliably engage in such prosocial gossip. In an experimental setting, 5-year-o ... Full text Cite

Preschoolers value those who sanction non-cooperators.

Journal Article Cognition · August 2016 Large-scale human cooperation among unrelated individuals requires the enforcement of social norms. However, such enforcement poses a problem because non-enforcers can free ride on others' costly and risky enforcement. One solution is that enforcers receiv ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young children (sometimes) do the right thing even when their peers do not

Journal Article Cognitive Development · July 1, 2016 Children must sometimes decide between conforming to peer behavior and doing what is right. While research shows that children have a strong inclination to act prosocially and to help conspecifics in need, many studies also demonstrate that children tend t ... Full text Open Access Cite

Great apes anticipate actions based on agents' (false) beliefs

Conference INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY · July 1, 2016 Link to item Cite

Extrinsic Rewards Diminish Costly Sharing in 3-Year-Olds.

Journal Article Child development · July 2016 Two studies investigated the influence of external rewards and social praise in young children's fairness-related behavior. The motivation of ninety-six 3-year-olds' to equalize unfair resource allocations was measured in three scenarios (collaboration, wi ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees coordinate in a snowdrift game

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · June 1, 2016 The snowdrift game is a model for studying social coordination in the context of competing interests. We presented pairs of chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, with a situation in which they could either pull a weighted tray together or pull alone to obtain food ... Full text Cite

Two- and 3-year-olds integrate linguistic and pedagogical cues in guiding inductive generalization and exploration.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · May 2016 Young children can in principle make generic inferences (e.g., "doffels are magnetic") on the basis of their own individual experience. Recent evidence, however, shows that by 4 years of age children make strong generic inferences on the basis of a single ... Full text Cite

Taking Turns or Not? Children's Approach to Limited Resource Problems in Three Different Cultures.

Journal Article Child development · May 2016 Some problems of resource distribution can be solved on equal terms only by taking turns. We presented such a problem to 168 pairs of 5- to 10-year-old children from one Western and two non-Western societies (German, Samburu, Kikuyu). Almost all German pai ... Full text Cite

Cultural Learning Redux.

Journal Article Child development · May 2016 M. Tomasello, A. Kruger, and H. Ratner (1993) proposed a theory of cultural learning comprising imitative learning, instructed learning, and collaborative learning. Empirical and theoretical advances in the past 20 years suggest modifications to the theory ... Full text Cite

Differing views: Can chimpanzees do Level 2 perspective-taking?

Journal Article Animal cognition · May 2016 Although chimpanzees understand what others may see, it is unclear whether they understand how others see things (Level 2 perspective-taking). We investigated whether chimpanzees can predict the behavior of a conspecific which is holding a mistaken perspec ... Full text Cite

Mixed NPs in German-English and German-Russian bilingual children

Chapter · April 25, 2016 Both cross-linguistic priming methodologies and research on codemixed utterances have been concerned with the nature of the underlying syntactic representations of bilinguals. The present paper investigated code-mixing at the morphosyntactic level (NP) by ... Cite

German Children’s Use of Word Order and Case Marking to Interpret Simple and Complex Sentences: Testing Differences Between Constructions and Lexical Items

Journal Article Language Learning and Development · April 2, 2016 ABSTRACT: Children and adults follow cues such as case marking and word order in their assignment of semantic roles in simple transitives (e.g., the dog chased the cat). It has been suggested that the same cues are used for the interpretation of complex se ... Full text Cite

The ontogeny of cultural learning

Journal Article Current Opinion in Psychology · April 1, 2016 All primates engage in one or another form of social learning. Humans engage in cultural learning. From very early in ontogeny human infants and young children do not just learn useful things from others, they conform to others in order to affiliate with t ... Full text Open Access Cite

Preschoolers understand the normativity of cooperatively structured competition.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2016 Human institutional practices often involve competition within a cooperative structure of mutually accepted rules. In a competitive game, for instance, we not only expect adherence to the rules of the game but also expect an opponent who tries to win and, ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young Children Understand the Role of Agreement in Establishing Arbitrary Norms-But Unanimity Is Key.

Journal Article Child development · March 2016 Human cultural groups value conformity to arbitrary norms (e.g., rituals, games) that are the result of collective "agreement." Ninety-six 3-year-olds had the opportunity to agree upon arbitrary norms with puppets. Results revealed that children normativel ... Full text Open Access Cite

Preschoolers use common ground in their justificatory reasoning with peers.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · March 2016 In the context of joint decision-making, we investigated whether preschoolers alter the informativeness of their justifications depending on the common ground that they share with their partner. Pairs of 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 146) were introduced to a no ... Full text Cite

Comprehension of iconic gestures by chimpanzees and human children.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · February 2016 Iconic gestures-communicative acts using hand or body movements that resemble their referent-figure prominently in theories of language evolution and development. This study contrasted the abilities of chimpanzees (N=11) and 4-year-old human children (N=24 ... Full text Cite

A Natural History of Human Morality

Book · January 4, 2016 Featured Publication Michael Tomasello offers the most detailed account to date of the evolution of human moral psychology. ... Cite

Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2016 Recent research on distributive justice suggests that young children prefer equal distributions. But sometimes unequal distributions are justified, such as when some individuals deserve more than others based on merit, need, or agreed-upon rules. When and ... Full text Open Access Cite

Giving Is Nicer than Taking: Preschoolers Reciprocate Based on the Social Intentions of the Distributor.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2016 Recent research has found that even preschoolers give more resources to others who have previously given resources to them, but the psychological bases of this reciprocity are unknown. In our study, a puppet distributed resources between herself and a chil ... Full text Open Access Cite

Precís of a natural history of human thinking

Journal Article Journal of Social Ontology · January 1, 2016 A précis of Michael Tomasello, A Natural History of Human Thinking (Harvard University Press, 2014). ... Full text Cite

Grammar

Chapter · January 1, 2016 To acquire competence with a natural language, young children must master the grammatical constructions of their language(s). In this article we outline the main theoretical issues in the field and trace the developmental path children follow from talking ... Full text Cite

Response to commentators

Journal Article Journal of Social Ontology · January 1, 2016 This paper is a reply to the comments by Henrike Moll, Glenda Satne, Ladislav Koreň and Michael Schmitz on Michael Tomasello, A Natural History of Human Thinking (Harvard University Press, 2014). ... Full text Cite

What Is a Group? Young Children's Perceptions of Different Types of Groups and Group Entitativity.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2016 To date, developmental research on groups has focused mainly on in-group biases and intergroup relations. However, little is known about children's general understanding of social groups and their perceptions of different forms of group. In this study, 5- ... Full text Cite

The effects of being watched on resource acquisition in chimpanzees and human children.

Journal Article Animal cognition · January 2016 Animals react in many different ways to being watched by others. In the context of cooperation, many theories emphasize reputational effects: Individuals should cooperate more if other potential cooperators are watching. In the context of competition, indi ... Full text Cite

Communication about absent entities in great apes and human infants.

Journal Article Cognition · December 2015 There is currently debate about the extent to which non-linguistic beings such as human infants and great apes are capable of absent reference. In a series of experiments we investigated the flexibility and specificity of great apes' (N=36) and 12 month-ol ... Full text Cite

How 18- and 24-month-old peers divide resources among themselves.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · December 2015 Young children are often considered "selfish" with resources because they are reluctant to give up things already in their possession (e.g., as in dictator games). In the current two studies, we presented pairs of 18- and 24-month-old toddlers with various ... Full text Cite

Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · December 2015 When it is not possible to distribute resources equitably to everyone, people look for an equitable or just procedure. In the current study, we investigated young children's sense of procedural justice. We tested 32 triads of 5-year-olds in a new resource ... Full text Cite

Uniquely human self-control begins at school age.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2015 Human beings have remarkable skills of self-control, but the evolutionary origins of these skills are unknown. Here we compare children at 3 and 6 years of age with one of humans' two nearest relatives, chimpanzees, on a battery of reactivity and self-cont ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young children use pedagogical cues to modulate the strength of normative inferences.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · November 2015 Young children understand pedagogical demonstrations as conveying generic, kind-relevant information. But, in some contexts, they also see almost any confident, intentional action on a novel artefact as normative and thus generic, regardless of whether thi ... Full text Cite

The effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on children's prosocial behavior, liking, affiliation, and trust.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · November 2015 Recent theoretical work has highlighted potential links between interpersonal collaboration and group membership in the evolution of human sociality. Here we compared the effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on young children's prosocial b ... Full text Cite

Michael Tomasello: Award for Distinguished Scientific Contributions.

Journal Article The American psychologist · November 2015 The APA Awards for Distinguished Scientific Contributions are presented to persons who, in the opinion of the Committee on Scientific Awards, have made distinguished theoretical or empirical contributions to basic research in psychology. One of the 2015 aw ... Full text Cite

Differences in the Ability of Apes and Children to Instruct Others Using Gestures

Journal Article Language Learning and Development · October 2, 2015 In all human cultures, people gesture iconically. However, the evolutionary basis of iconic gestures is unknown. In this study, chimpanzees and bonobos, and 2- and 3-year-old children, learned how to operate two apparatuses to get rewards. Then, at test, o ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees strategically manipulate what others can see.

Journal Article Animal cognition · September 2015 Humans often strategically manipulate the informational access of others to their own advantage. Although chimpanzees know what others can and cannot see, it is unclear whether they can strategically manipulate others' visual access. In this study, chimpan ... Full text Cite

Young children use shared experience to interpret definite reference.

Journal Article Journal of child language · September 2015 We investigated whether children at the ages of two and three years understand that a speaker's use of the definite article specifies a referent that is in common ground between speaker and listener. An experimenter and a child engaged in joint actions in ... Full text Cite

The relationship between infant holdout and gives, and pointing

Journal Article Infancy · September 1, 2015 We provide an analysis of holdout and giving (Ho&G) behaviours in prelinguistic infants and investigate their relationship with index finger pointing. The frequency of Ho&Gs at 10 and 11 months along with the length of the following social interaction corr ... Full text Cite

Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words

Journal Article Frontiers in Psychology · August 11, 2015 When children are learning a novel object label, they tend to exclude as possible referents familiar objects for which they already have a name. In the current study, we wanted to know if children would behave in this same way regardless of how well they k ... Full text Cite

Fair Is Not Fair Everywhere.

Journal Article Psychological science · August 2015 Distributing the spoils of a joint enterprise on the basis of work contribution or relative productivity seems natural to the modern Western mind. But such notions of merit-based distributive justice may be culturally constructed norms that vary with the s ... Full text Open Access Cite

Productivity of Noun Slots in Verb Frames.

Journal Article Cognitive science · August 2015 Productivity is a central concept in the study of language and language acquisition. As a test case for exploring the notion of productivity, we focus on the noun slots of verb frames, such as __want__, __see__, and __get__. We develop a novel combination ... Full text Cite

Focusing and shifting attention in human children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · August 2015 Humans often must coordinate co-occurring activities, and their flexible skills for doing so would seem to be uniquely powerful. In 2 studies, we compared 4- and 5-year-old children and one of humans' nearest relatives, chimpanzees, in their ability to foc ... Full text Cite

Novel paradigms to measure variability of behavior in early childhood: posture, gaze, and pupil dilation

Journal Article Frontiers in Psychology · July 9, 2015 A central challenge of investigating the underlying mechanisms of and the individual differences in young children’s behavior is the measurement of the internal physiological mechanism and the involved expressive emotions. Here, we illustrate two paradigms ... Full text Cite

The goggles experiment: Can chimpanzees use self-experience to infer what a competitor can see?

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · July 1, 2015 In two experiments, we investigated whether chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, can use self-experience to infer what another sees. Subjects first gained self-experience with the visual properties of an object (either opaque or see-through). In a subsequent test ... Full text Open Access Cite

Teaching versus enforcing game rules in preschoolers' peer interactions.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · July 2015 Children use normative language in two key contexts: when teaching others and when enforcing social norms. We presented pairs of 3- and 5-year-old peers (N=192) with a sorting game in two experimental conditions (in addition to a third baseline condition). ... Full text Cite

Late Emergence of the First Possession Heuristic: Evidence From a Small-Scale Culture

Journal Article Child Development · July 1, 2015 Western preschool children often assign ownership based on first possession and some theorists have proposed that this judgment might be an early emerging, innate bias. Five- to 9-year-olds (n = 112) from a small-scale group in Kenya (Kikuyu) watched video ... Full text Cite

Restorative Justice in Children.

Journal Article Current biology : CB · June 2015 An important, and perhaps uniquely human, mechanism for maintaining cooperation against free riders is third-party punishment. Our closest living relatives, chimpanzees, will not punish third parties even though they will do so when personally affected. Un ... Full text Cite

Children use salience to solve coordination problems.

Journal Article Developmental science · May 2015 Humans are routinely required to coordinate with others. When communication is not possible, adults often achieve this by using salient cues in the environment (e.g. going to the Eiffel Tower, as an obvious meeting point). To explore the development of thi ... Full text Open Access Cite

Young Children’s Intonational Marking of New, Given and Contrastive Referents

Journal Article Language Learning and Development · April 3, 2015 In the current study we investigate whether 2- and 3-year-old German children use intonation productively to mark the informational status of referents. Using a story-telling task, we compared children’s and adults’ intonational realization via pitch accen ... Full text Cite

Young children show the bystander effect in helping situations.

Journal Article Psychological science · April 2015 Much research in social psychology has shown that otherwise helpful people often fail to help when bystanders are present. Research in developmental psychology has shown that even very young children help and that the presence of others can actually increa ... Full text Cite

Two-year-old children but not domestic dogs understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze.

Journal Article Developmental science · March 2015 Infants can see someone pointing to one of two buckets and infer that the toy they are seeking is hidden inside. Great apes do not succeed in this task, but, surprisingly, domestic dogs do. However, whether children and dogs understand these communicative ... Full text Cite

Conforming to coordinate: children use majority information for peer coordination.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · March 2015 Humans are constantly required to coordinate their behaviour with others. As this often relies on everyone's convergence on the same strategy (e.g., driving on the left side of the road), a common solution is to conform to majority behaviour. In this study ... Full text Cite

18-month-olds comprehend indirect communicative acts.

Journal Article Cognition · March 2015 From soon after their first birthdays young children are able to make inferences from a communicator's referential act (e.g., pointing to a container) to her overall social goal for communication (e.g., to inform that a searched-for toy is inside; see Behn ... Full text Cite

Preschoolers' understanding of the role of communication and cooperation in establishing property rights.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · February 2015 Property as a social "agreement" comprises both a communicative component, in which someone makes a claim that she is entitled to some piece of property, and a cooperative component, in which others in the community respect that claim as legitimate. In the ... Full text Open Access Cite

Chimpanzees trust conspecifics to engage in low-cost reciprocity.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · February 2015 Many of humans' most important social interactions rely on trust, including most notably among strangers. But little is known about the evolutionary roots of human trust. We presented chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with a modified version of the human trust ... Full text Cite

Restorative justice in young children

Journal Article Current Biology · 2015 Featured Publication Open Access Cite

"I know you don't know I know…" children use second-order false-belief reasoning for peer coordination.

Journal Article Child development · January 2015 Numerous studies have investigated children's abilities to attribute mental states, but few have examined their ability to recruit these abilities in social interactions. Here, 6-year-olds (N = 104) were tested on whether they can use first- and second-ord ... Full text Open Access Cite

Production and Comprehension of Gestures between Orang-Utans (Pongo pygmaeus) in a Referential Communication Game.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2015 Orang-utans played a communication game in two studies testing their ability to produce and comprehend requestive pointing. While the 'communicator' could see but not obtain hidden food, the 'donor' could release the food to the communicator, but could not ... Full text Cite

Non-egalitarian allocations among preschool peers in a face-to-face bargaining task.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2015 In face-to-face bargaining tasks human adults almost always agree on an equal split of resources. This is due to mutually recognized fairness and equality norms. Early developmental studies on sharing and equality norms found that egalitarian allocations o ... Full text Cite

Children conform to the behavior of peers; other great apes stick with what they know.

Journal Article Psychological science · December 2014 All primates learn things from conspecifics socially, but it is not clear whether they conform to the behavior of these conspecifics--if conformity is defined as overriding individually acquired behavioral tendencies in order to copy peers' behavior. In th ... Full text Cite

Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and human children in a Stag Hunt game.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · December 2014 Much of human cooperation takes place in mutualistic contexts in which the main challenge for individuals is how to coordinate decisions. In the current studies, we compared the abilities of chimpanzees and young children to coordinate with a partner in tw ... Full text Cite

All great ape species (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) and two-and-a-half-year-old children (Homo sapiens) discriminate appearance from reality.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · November 2014 Nonhuman great apes and human children were tested for an understanding that appearance does not always correspond to reality. Subjects were 29 great apes (bonobos [Pan paniscus], chimpanzees [Pan troglodytes], gorillas [Gorilla gorilla], and orangutans [P ... Full text Cite

Young children create partner-specific referential pacts with peers.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · October 2014 In 2 studies, we investigated how peers establish a referential pact to call something, for example, a cushion versus a pillow (both equally felicitous). In Study 1, pairs of 4- and 6-year-old German-speaking peers established a referential pact for an art ... Full text Cite

Reasoning during joint decision-making by preschool peers

Journal Article Cognitive Development · October 1, 2014 Reasoning with a peer to make a joint decision involves making a proposal (e.g., "Polar bears go here") and justifying it with relevant facts (e.g., "This is ice") based on common ground assumptions or warrants (e.g., polar bears need ice). Twenty-four dya ... Full text Cite

Limitations to the cultural ratchet effect in young children.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · October 2014 Although many animal species show at least some evidence of cultural transmission, broadly defined, only humans show clear evidence of cumulative culture. In the current study, we investigated whether young children show the "ratchet effect," an important ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) instrumentally help but do not communicate in a mutualistic cooperative task.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · August 2014 Chimpanzees cooperate in a variety of contexts, but communicating to influence and regulate cooperative activities is rare. It is unclear whether this reflects chimpanzees' general inability or whether they have found other means to coordinate cooperative ... Full text Cite

Young children create iconic gestures to inform others.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · August 2014 Much is known about young children's use of deictic gestures such as pointing. Much less is known about their use of other types of communicative gestures, especially iconic or symbolic gestures. In particular, it is unknown whether children can create ico ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of denial.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · August 2014 Although a fair amount is known about young children's production of negation, little is known about their comprehension. Here, we focus on arguably the most complex basic form, denial, and how young children understand denial, when it is expressed in resp ... Full text Cite

The oxidative environment: a mediator of interspecies communication that drives symbiosis evolution.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · June 2014 Symbiotic interactions are ubiquitous in nature and play a major role in driving the evolution of life. Interactions between partners are often mediated by shared signalling pathways, which strongly influence both partners' biology and the evolution of the ... Full text Cite

Children's norm enforcement in their interactions with peers.

Journal Article Child development · May 2014 This study investigates how children negotiate social norms with peers. In Study 1, 48 pairs of 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 96) and in Study 2, 48 pairs of 5- and 7-year-olds (N = 96) were presented with sorting tasks with conflicting instructions (one child b ... Full text Cite

The communicative contexts of grammatical aspect use in English.

Journal Article Journal of child language · May 2014 In many of the world's languages grammatical aspect is used to indicate how events unfold over time. In English, activities that are ongoing can be distinguished from those that are completed using the morphological marker -ing. Using naturalistic observat ... Full text Cite

Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes.

Journal Article Developmental psychobiology · April 2014 There is very little research comparing great ape and human cognition developmentally. In the current studies we compared a cross-sectional sample of 2- to 4-year-old human children (n=48) with a large sample of chimpanzees and bonobos in the same age rang ... Full text Open Access Cite

Differences in the nonverbal requests of great apes and human infants.

Journal Article Child development · March 2014 This study investigated how great apes and human infants use imperative pointing to request objects. In a series of three experiments (infants, N = 44; apes, N = 12), subjects were given the opportunity to either point to a desired object from a distance o ... Full text Cite

Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2014 Recent studies suggest that infants understand that others can have false beliefs. However, most of these studies have used looking time measures, and the few that have used behavioral measures are all based on the change-of-location paradigm, leading to c ... Full text Cite

A Natural History of Human Thinking

Book · February 2014 Featured Publication Tool-making or culture, language or religious belief: ever since Darwin, thinkers have struggled to identify what fundamentally differentiates human beings from other animals. In this much-anticipated book, Michael Tomasello weaves his twenty years of comp ... Link to item Cite

Discourse particles and belief reasoning: The case of German doch

Journal Article Journal of Semantics · February 1, 2014 Discourse particles typically express the attitudes of interlocutors with respect to the propositional content of an utterance - for example, marking whether or not a speaker believes the content of the proposition that she uttered. In German, the particle ... Full text Cite

The ultra-social animal

Journal Article European Journal of Social Psychology · January 1, 2014 Featured Publication In evolutionary perspective, what is most remarkable about human sociality is its many and diverse forms of cooperation. Here, I provide an overview of some recent research, mostly from our laboratory, comparing human children with their nearest living rel ... Full text Open Access Cite

The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, volume ii classic edition

Book · January 1, 2014 From the point of view of psychology and cognitive science, much of modern linguistics is too formal and mathematical to be of much use. The New Psychology of Language volumes broke new ground by introducing functional and cognitive approaches to language ... Cite

The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, volume I classic edition

Book · January 1, 2014 From the point of view of psychology and cognitive science, much of modern linguistics is too formal and mathematical to be of much use. The New Psychology of Language volumes broke new ground by introducing functional and cognitive approaches to language ... Cite

Side matters: Potential mechanisms underlying dogs' performance in a social eavesdropping paradigm

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2014 Social eavesdropping is the gathering of information by observing interactions between other individuals. Previous studies have claimed that dogs, Canis familiaris, are able to use information obtained via social eavesdropping, that is, preferring a genero ... Full text Cite

Preschoolers are sensitive to free riding in a public goods game

Journal Article Frontiers in Psychology · January 1, 2014 Despite the benefits of cooperation, selfish individuals often produce outcomes where everyone is worse off. This "tragedy of the commons" has been demonstrated experimentally in adults with the public goods game. Contributions to a public good decline ove ... Full text Cite

Generalize or personalize--do dogs transfer an acquired rule to novel situations and persons?

Journal Article PloS one · January 2014 Recent studies have raised the question of whether dogs, like human infants, comprehend an established rule as generalizable, normative knowledge or rather as episodic information, existing only in the immediate situation. In the current study we tested wh ... Full text Cite

Young children's planning in a collaborative problem-solving task

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2014 One important component of collaborative problem solving is the ability to plan one's own action in relation to that of a partner. We presented 3- and 5-year-old peer pairs with two different tool choice situations in which they had to choose complementary ... Full text Cite

Dueling dualists : Commentary on carpendale, atwood, and kettner

Journal Article Human Development · January 1, 2014 Full text Cite

Do domestic dogs learn words based on humans' referential behaviour?

Journal Article PloS one · January 2014 Some domestic dogs learn to comprehend human words, although the nature and basis of this learning is unknown. In the studies presented here we investigated whether dogs learn words through an understanding of referential actions by humans rather than simp ... Full text Cite

Does sympathy motivate prosocial behaviour in great apes?

Journal Article PloS one · January 2014 Prosocial behaviours such as helping, comforting, or sharing are central to human social life. Because they emerge early in ontogeny, it has been proposed that humans are prosocial by nature and that from early on empathy and sympathy motivate such behavio ... Full text Cite

Young children's creation and transmission of social norms

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2014 Children's lives are governed by social norms. Since Piaget, however, it has been assumed that they understand very little about how norms work. Recent studies in which children enforce social norms indicate a richer understanding, but children are still r ... Full text Cite

Two- and 3-Year-Olds Know What Others Have and Have Not Heard

Journal Article Journal of Cognition and Development · January 1, 2014 Recent studies have established that even infants can determine what others know based on previous visual experience. In the current study, we investigated whether 2- and 3-year-olds know what others know based on previous auditory experience. A child and ... Full text Cite

Familiar verbs are not always easier than novel verbs: how German pre-school children comprehend active and passive sentences.

Journal Article Cognitive science · January 2014 Many studies show a developmental advantage for transitive sentences with familiar verbs over those with novel verbs. It might be that once familiar verbs become entrenched in particular constructions, they would be more difficult to understand (than would ... Full text Cite

Meritocratic sharing is based on collaboration in 3-year-olds.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · January 2014 The present study investigated young preschoolers' proportional allocation of rewards in 2 different work contexts. We presented 32 pairs of 3.5-year-old peers with a collaborative task to obtain rewards by pulling ropes. In order to establish differences ... Full text Cite

How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic material?

Journal Article Developmental psychology · December 2013 In 3 studies we explored when 3-year-olds would imitate novel words in utterances produced by adult speakers. Child and experimenter took turns in requesting objects from a game master. The experimenter always went first and always preceded the object's fa ... Full text Cite

Young children understand and defend the entitlements of others.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · December 2013 Human social life is structured by social norms creating both obligations and entitlements. Recent research has found that young children enforce simple obligations against norm violators by protesting. It is not known, however, whether they understand ent ... Full text Cite

Three-year-olds hide their communicative intentions in appropriate contexts.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2013 Human cooperative communication involves both an informative intention that the recipient understands the content of the signal and also a (Gricean) communicative intention that the recipient recognizes that the speaker has an informative intention. The de ... Full text Open Access Cite

3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect verbal communication.

Journal Article Child development · November 2013 Three studies investigated 3-year-old children's ability to determine a speaker's communicative intent when the speaker's overt utterance related to that intent only indirectly. Studies 1 and 2 examined children's comprehension of indirectly stated request ... Full text Cite

Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup members and potential reciprocators.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2013 Human cooperation depends on individuals caring about their reputation, and so they sometimes attempt to manage them strategically. Here we show that even 5-year-old children strategically manage their reputation. In an experimental setting, children share ... Full text Cite

Five-year-olds understand fair as equal in a mini-ultimatum game.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · October 2013 In studies of children's resource distribution, it is almost always the case that "fair" means an equal amount for all. In the mini-ultimatum game, players are confronted with situations in which fair does not always mean equal, and so the recipient of an ... Full text Cite

The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · October 2013 Contingent reciprocity is important in theories of the evolution of human cooperation, but it has been very little studied in ontogeny. We gave 2- and 3-year-old children the opportunity to either help or share with a partner after that partner either had ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, recognize successful actions, but fail toimitate them

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · October 1, 2013 Cultural transmission, by definition, involves some form of social learning. Chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates clearly engage in some forms of social learning enabling some types of cultural transmission, but there is controversy about whether they c ... Full text Cite

Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants.

Journal Article Child development · September 2013 Most previous research on imitation in infancy has focused on infants' learning of instrumental actions on objects. This study focused instead on the more social side of imitation, testing whether being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in infants, as ... Full text Cite

Non-verbal communication enables children's coordination in a "Stag Hunt" game

Journal Article European Journal of Developmental Psychology · September 1, 2013 This study assessed the role of non-verbal communication in 4-year-old children's decisions to coordinate with others. During a "Stag Hunt" game, the child and an adult individually and continually collected low-value prizes (hares). Occasionally, an alter ... Full text Cite

Subject and object omission in children's early transitive constructions: A discourse-pragmatic approach

Journal Article Applied Psycholinguistics · July 25, 2013 This paper investigates discourse effects on the provision of both subjects and objects and investigates whether pragmatic discourse features govern the realization/omission of both constituents alike. In an elicitation study, we examined how the discourse ... Full text Cite

The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures.

Journal Article Animal cognition · July 2013 Great apes communicate with gestures in flexible ways. Based on several lines of evidence, Tomasello and colleagues have posited that many of these gestures are learned via ontogenetic ritualization-a process of mutual anticipation in which particular soci ... Full text Cite

The attention-grammar interface: Eye-gaze cues structural choice in children and adults

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · July 1, 2013 We investigated whether children (3- and 4-year-olds) and adults can use the active passive alternation - essentially a choice of subject - in a way that is consistent with the eye-gaze of the speaker. Previous work suggests the function of the subject pos ... Full text Cite

Young children sympathize less in response to unjustified emotional distress.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · June 2013 Three-year-old children saw an adult displaying the exact same distress in 3 different conditions: (a) the adult's distress was appropriate to a genuine harm, (b) the adult's distress was an overreaction to a minor inconvenience, and (c) there was no appar ... Full text Cite

Error patterns in young German children's wh-questions.

Journal Article Journal of child language · June 2013 In this article we report two studies: a detailed longitudinal analysis of errors in wh-questions from six German-learning children (age 2 ; 0-3 ; 0) and an analysis of the prosodic characteristics of wh-questions in German child-directed speech. The resul ... Full text Cite

Three-year-olds understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze

Journal Article Interaction Studies · May 13, 2013 The communicative interactions of very young children almost always involve language (based on conventions), gesture (based on bodily deixis or iconicity) and directed gaze. In this study, ninety-six children (3;0 years) were asked to determine the locatio ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs conceal auditory but not visual information from others.

Journal Article Animal cognition · May 2013 A number of studies have shown that dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective, but it remains unclear whether they use an egocentric strategy to assess what humans perceive. We investigated whether dogs know what a human can see and hear, even when the d ... Full text Cite

Parental Presence and Encouragement Do Not Influence Helping in Young Children

Journal Article Infancy · May 1, 2013 Young children begin helping others with simple instrumental problems from soon after their first birthdays. In previous observations of this phenomenon, both naturalistic and experimental, children's parents were in the room and could potentially have inf ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzee responders still behave like rational maximizers.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · May 2013 Full text Cite

Dogs steal in the dark.

Journal Article Animal cognition · May 2013 All current evidence of visual perspective taking in dogs can possibly be explained by dogs reacting to certain stimuli rather than understanding what others see. In the current study, we set up a situation in which contextual information and social cues a ... Full text Cite

Do domestic dogs interpret pointing as a command?

Journal Article Animal cognition · May 2013 Domestic dogs comprehend human gestural communication flexibly, particularly the pointing gesture. Here, we examine whether dogs interpret pointing informatively, that is, as simply providing information, or rather as a command, for example, ordering them ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task.

Journal Article Biology letters · April 2013 Many animal species cooperate, but the underlying proximate mechanisms are often unclear. We presented chimpanzees with a mutualistic collaborative food-retrieval task requiring complementary roles, and tested subjects' ability to help their partner perfor ... Full text Cite

Taking versus confronting visual perspectives in preschool children.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · April 2013 Recent evidence suggests that 3-year-olds can take other people's visual perspectives not only when they perceive different things (Level 1) but even when they see the same thing differently (Level 2). One hypothesis is that 3-year-olds are good perspectiv ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task.

Journal Article Animal cognition · March 2013 Cooperative hunting is a cognitively challenging activity since individuals have to coordinate movements with a partner and at the same time react to the prey. Domestic dogs evolved from wolves, who engage in cooperative hunting regularly, but it is not cl ... Full text Cite

Children's ability to answer different types of questions.

Journal Article Journal of child language · March 2013 Young children answer many questions every day. The extent to which they do this in an adult-like way - following Grice's Maxim of Quantity by providing the requested information, no more no less - has been studied very little. In an experiment, we found t ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of cultural common ground.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · March 2013 Human social interaction depends on individuals identifying the common ground they have with others, based both on personally shared experiences and on cultural common ground that all members of the group share. We introduced 3- and 5-year-old children to ... Full text Cite

Allocation of resources to collaborators and free-riders in 3-year-olds.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · February 2013 Recent studies have shown that in situations where resources have been acquired collaboratively, children at around 3 years of age share mostly equally. We investigated 3-year-olds' sharing behavior with a collaborating partner and a free-riding partner wh ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees predict that a competitor's preference will match their own.

Journal Article Biology letters · February 2013 The ability to predict how another individual will behave is useful in social competition. Chimpanzees can predict the behaviour of another based on what they observe her to see, hear, know and infer. Here we show that chimpanzees act on the assumption tha ... Full text Cite

Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · February 2013 Humans make decisions about when and with whom to cooperate based on their reputations. People either learn about others by direct interaction or by observing third-party interactions or gossip. An important question is whether other animal species, especi ... Full text Open Access Cite

Origins of human cooperation and morality

Journal Article Annual Review of Psychology · January 1, 2013 Featured Publication From an evolutionary perspective, morality is a form of cooperation. Cooperation requires individuals either to suppress their own self-interest or to equate it with that of others. We review recent research on the origins of human morality, both phylogene ... Full text Open Access Cite

The early ontogeny of human cooperation and morality

Chapter · January 1, 2013 The seminal work in the modern study of children’s moral development is Piaget’s (1932/1997) The Moral Judgment of the Child. As is well known, Piaget claimed that before the age of 8 or 9 years children make moral judgments based only on a respect for aut ... Full text Cite

Three-year-olds' understanding of the consequences of joint commitments.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2013 Here we investigate the extent of children's understanding of the joint commitments inherent in joint activities. Three-year-old children either made a joint commitment to assemble a puzzle with a puppet partner, or else the child and puppet each assembled ... Full text Cite

A New Look at Children's Prosocial Motivation

Journal Article Infancy · January 1, 2013 Young children routinely behave prosocially, but what is their motivation for doing so? Here, we review three studies which show that young children (1) are intrinsically motivated rather than motivated by extrinsic rewards; (2) are more inclined to help t ... Full text Cite

Bonobos, Pan paniscus, chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, prefer to feed alone

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2013 Many primates share food, but the motives behind this food sharing are mostly not known. We investigated individuals' preference to feed either alone or together with a tolerant partner. Subjects (chimpanzees who are highly competitive around food, bonobos ... Full text Cite

Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food?

Journal Article Animal cognition · January 2013 Although many studies have investigated domestic dogs' (Canis familiaris) use of human communicative cues, little is known about their use of humans' emotional expressions. We conducted a study following the general paradigm of Repacholi in Dev Psychol 34: ... Full text Cite

Symposium Communicative Intentions in the Mind/Brain

Conference Cooperative Minds: Social Interaction and Group Dynamics - Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, CogSci 2013 · January 1, 2013 Cite

Language and Gesture Evolution

Conference Cooperative Minds: Social Interaction and Group Dynamics - Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, CogSci 2013 · January 1, 2013 Cite

Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence Hypothesis

Journal Article Current Anthropology · December 1, 2012 Modern theories of the evolution of human cooperation focus mainly on altruism. In contrast, we propose that humans' species-unique forms of cooperation-as well as their species-unique forms of cognition, communication, and social life-all derive from mutu ... Full text Cite

Theft in an ultimatum game: chimpanzees and bonobos are insensitive to unfairness.

Journal Article Biology letters · December 2012 Humans, but not chimpanzees, punish unfair offers in ultimatum games, suggesting that fairness concerns evolved sometime after the split between the lineages that gave rise to Homo and Pan. However, nothing is known about fairness concerns in the other Pan ... Full text Cite

Why be nice? Better not think about it.

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · December 2012 Are people more likely to be cooperative if they must act quickly or if they have more time to mull it over? The results of a recent series of studies suggest that peoples' initial impulse is to cooperate, but that with more time and reflection they become ... Full text Cite

How chimpanzees solve collective action problems.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · December 2012 We presented small groups of chimpanzees with two collective action situations, in which action was necessary for reward but there was a disincentive for individuals to act owing to the possibility of free-riding on the efforts of others. We found that in ... Full text Cite

Origins of the human pointing gesture: a training study.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2012 Despite its importance in the development of children's skills of social cognition and communication, very little is known about the ontogenetic origins of the pointing gesture. We report a training study in which mothers gave children one month of extra d ... Full text Cite

Great apes infer others' goals based on context.

Journal Article Animal cognition · November 2012 In previous studies claiming to demonstrate that great apes understand the goals of others, the apes could potentially have been using subtle behavioral cues present during the test to succeed. In the current studies, we ruled out the use of such cues by m ... Full text Cite

Cognitive Linguistics and First Language Acquisition

Chapter · September 18, 2012 This article reviews some of the best-known and most interesting work on language acquisition from within the framework of functional-cognitive linguistics, particularly those on meaning and conceptualization as well as usage and grammar (grammatical const ... Full text Cite

The ontogenetic origins of human cooperation

Chapter · September 18, 2012 This article suggests that the ontogenesis of shared intentionality depends on the developmentally primitive phenomenon of 'joint attention'. This is the ability of the infant to understand that they and other individuals can attend to the same object and ... Full text Cite

Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others helped.

Journal Article Psychological science · September 2012 Young children help other people, but it is not clear why. In the current study, we found that 2-year-old children's sympathetic arousal, as measured by relative changes in pupil dilation, is similar when they themselves help a person and when they see tha ... Full text Cite

No third-party punishment in chimpanzees.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · September 2012 Punishment can help maintain cooperation by deterring free-riding and cheating. Of particular importance in large-scale human societies is third-party punishment in which individuals punish a transgressor or norm violator even when they themselves are not ... Full text Cite

Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator's group affiliation.

Journal Article Cognition · September 2012 To become cooperative members of their cultural groups, developing children must follow their group's social norms. But young children are not just blind norm followers, they are also active norm enforcers, for example, protesting and correcting when someo ... Full text Cite

Semantics of the transitive construction: prototype effects and developmental comparisons.

Journal Article Cognitive science · September 2012 This paper investigates whether an abstract linguistic construction shows the kind of prototype effects characteristic of non-linguistic categories, in both adults and young children. Adapting the prototype-plus-distortion methodology of Franks and Bransfo ... Full text Cite

Twelve-month-olds' comprehension and production of pointing.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · September 2012 This study explored whether infants aged 12 months already recognize the communicative function of pointing gestures. Infants participated in a task requiring them to comprehend an adult's informative pointing gesture to the location of a hidden toy. They ... Full text Cite

Young Children Enforce Social Norms

Journal Article Current Directions in Psychological Science · August 1, 2012 Social norms have played a key role in the evolution of human cooperation, serving to stabilize prosocial and egalitarian behavior despite the self-serving motives of individuals. Young children's behavior mostly conforms to social norms, as they follow ad ... Full text Cite

Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing

Journal Article First Language · August 1, 2012 The prototypical word learning situation in western, middle-class cultures is dyadic: an adult addresses a child directly, ideally in a manner sensitive to their current focus of attention. But young children also seem to learn many of their words in polya ... Full text Cite

Three-year-olds understand appearance and reality--just not about the same object at the same time.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · July 2012 Young children struggle in the classic tests of appearance versus reality. In the current Study 1, 3-year-olds had to determine which of 2 objects (a deceptive or a nondeceptive one) an adult requested when asking for the "real X" versus "the one that look ... Full text Cite

How two word-trained dogs integrate pointing and naming.

Journal Article Animal cognition · July 2012 Two word-trained dogs were presented with acts of reference in which a human pointed, named objects, or simultaneously did both. The question was whether these dogs would assume co-reference of pointing and naming and thus pick the pointed-to object. Resul ... Full text Cite

Differences in cognitive processes underlying the collaborative activities of children and chimpanzees

Journal Article Cognitive Development · April 1, 2012 We compared the performance of 3- and 5-year-old children with that of chimpanzees in two tasks requiring collaboration via complementary roles. In both tasks, children and chimpanzees were able to coordinate two complementary roles with peers and solve th ... Full text Cite

Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans.

Journal Article Current biology : CB · April 2012 Cultural transmission is a key component of human evolution. Two of humans' closest living relatives, chimpanzees and orangutans, have also been argued to transmit behavioral traditions across generations culturally [1-3], but how much the process might re ... Full text Cite

Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication.

Journal Article Topics in cognitive science · April 2012 Children often refer to things ambiguously but learn not to from responding to clarification requests. We review and explore this learning process here. In Study 1, eighty-four 2- and 4-year-olds were tested for their ability to request stickers from eithe ... Full text Cite

Do chimpanzees know what others see-or only what they are looking at?

Chapter · March 22, 2012 This chapter examines the distinction between two approaches in interpreting the behaviour in non-human animals. It explains the 'boosters' interpret behaviour in psychologically rich ways while 'scoffers' prefer psychologically lean interpretations. It co ... Full text Cite

Linguistic Communication and Social Understanding

Chapter · March 22, 2012 This chapter explores the early stages of pragmatic language acquisition before taking up the issue of syntax and semantics. It suggests that the relation between language and theory of mind is different depending on which aspect of social understanding is ... Full text Cite

How dogs know when communication is intended for them.

Journal Article Developmental science · March 2012 Domestic dogs comprehend human gestural communication in a way that other animal species do not. But little is known about the specific cues they use to determine when human communication is intended for them. In a series of four studies, we confronted bot ... Full text Cite

The acquisition of the active transitive construction in English: A detailed case study

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · February 1, 2012 In this study, we test a number of predictions concerning children's knowledge of the transitive Subject-Verb-Object (SVO) construction between two and three years on one child (Thomas) for whom we have densely collected data. The data show that the earlie ... Full text Cite

Dogs (Canis familiaris) evaluate humans on the basis of direct experiences only.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2012 Reputation formation is a key component in the social interactions of many animal species. An evaluation of reputation is drawn from two principal sources: direct experience of an individual and indirect experience from observing that individual interactin ... Full text Cite

Can we dissociate contingency learning from social learning in word acquisition by 24-month-olds?

Journal Article PloS one · January 2012 We compared 24-month-old children's learning when their exposure to words came either in an interactive (coupled) context or in a nonsocial (decoupled) context. We measured the children's learning with two different methods: one in which they were asked to ... Full text Cite

One-year-old infants follow others' voice direction.

Journal Article Psychological science · January 2012 We investigated 1-year-old infants' ability to infer an adult's focus of attention solely on the basis of her voice direction. In Studies 1 and 2, 12- and 16-month-olds watched an adult go behind a barrier and then heard her verbally express excitement abo ... Full text Cite

Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2012 Virtually all theories of the evolution of cooperation require that cooperators find ways to interact with one another selectively, to the exclusion of cheaters. This means that individuals must make reputational judgments about others as cooperators, base ... Full text Cite

Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2012 BackgroundSocial learning research in apes has focused on social learning in the technical (problem solving) domain - an approach that confounds action and physical information. Successful subjects in such studies may have been able to perform tar ... Full text Cite

Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2012 Chimpanzees routinely follow the gaze of humans to outside targets. However, in most studies using object choice they fail to use communicative gestures (e.g. pointing) to find hidden food. Chimpanzees' failure to do this may be due to several difficulties ... Full text Cite

Collaboration in young children.

Journal Article Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006) · January 2012 Humans accomplish much of what they do in collaboration with others. In ontogeny, children's earliest abilities to collaborate develop in two basic steps. First, 1- and 2-year-olds learn to form with others joint goals and joint attention--which include an ... Full text Cite

Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children's understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2012 Some children's social activities are structured by joint goals. In previous research, the criterion used to determine this was relatively weak: if the partner stopped interacting, did the child attempt to re-engage her? But re-engagement attempts could ea ... Full text Cite

Children's developing commitments to joint goals.

Journal Article Child development · January 2012 This study investigated young children's commitment to a joint goal by assessing whether peers in collaborative activities continue to collaborate until all received their rewards. Forty-eight 2.5- and 3.5-year-old children worked on an apparatus dyadicall ... Full text Cite

Two-year-old children differentiate test questions from genuine questions.

Journal Article Journal of child language · January 2012 Children are frequently confronted with so-called 'test questions'. While genuine questions are requests for missing information, test questions ask for information obviously already known to the questioner. In this study we explored whether two-year-old c ... Full text Cite

Methodological challenges in the study of primate cognition

Journal Article Science · December 2, 2011 Laboratory studies of primate cognition face the problem that captive populations of a species are not always comparable, and generalizations to natural populations are never certain. Studies of primate cognition in the field face the problem that replicat ... Full text Cite

Coordination of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a Stag Hunt Game

Journal Article International Journal of Primatology · December 1, 2011 Group-living animals frequently face situations in which they must coordinate individual and sometimes conflicting goals. We assessed chimpanzees' ability to coordinate in a Stag Hunt game. Dyads were confronted with a situation in which each individual wa ... Full text Cite

Children aged 2 ; 1 use transitive syntax to make a semantic-role interpretation in a pointing task.

Journal Article Journal of child language · November 2011 The current study used a forced choice pointing paradigm to examine whether English children aged 2 ; 1 can use abstract knowledge of the relationship between word order position and semantic roles to make an active behavioural decision when interpreting a ... Full text Cite

Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children.

Journal Article Child development · November 2011 Both adults and adolescents often conform their behavior and opinions to peer groups, even when they themselves know better. The current study investigated this phenomenon in 24 groups of 4 children between 4;2 and 4;9 years of age. Children often made the ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, prefer individual over collaborative strategies towards goals

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · November 1, 2011 Chimpanzees engage in a number of group activities, but it is still unclear to what extent they prefer mutualistic collaborative strategies over individual strategies to achieve their goals. In one experiment, we gave chimpanzees the choice between pulling ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of violations of property rights.

Journal Article Cognition · November 2011 The present work investigated young children's normative understanding of property rights using a novel methodology. Two- and 3-year-old children participated in situations in which an actor (1) took possession of an object for himself, and (2) attempted t ... Full text Cite

A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2011 The adaptive behavior of primates, including humans, is often mediated by temperament. Human behavior likely differs from that of other primates in part due to temperament. In the current study we compared the reaction of bonobos, chimpanzees, orangutans, ... Full text Open Access Cite

Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate.

Journal Article Current biology : CB · October 2011 Human societies are built on collaborative activities. Already from early childhood, human children are skillful and proficient collaborators. They recognize when they need help in solving a problem and actively recruit collaborators [1, 2]. The societies ... Full text Cite

Dogs, Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request but not to inform

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · October 1, 2011 Dogs are especially skilful at comprehending human communicative signals. This raises the question of whether they are also able to produce such signals flexibly, specifically, whether they helpfully produce indicative ('showing') behaviours to inform an i ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of markedness in non-verbal communication.

Journal Article Journal of child language · September 2011 Speakers often anticipate how recipients will interpret their utterances. If they wish some other, less obvious interpretation, they may 'mark' their utterance (e.g. with special intonations or facial expressions). We investigated whether two- and three-ye ... Full text Cite

The role of perceptual availability and discourse context in young children's question answering.

Journal Article Journal of child language · September 2011 Three- and four-year-old children were asked predicate-focus questions ('What's X doing?') about a scene in which an agent performed an action on a patient. We varied: (i) whether (or not) the preceding discourse context, which established the patient as g ... Full text Cite

Young children's responses to guilt displays.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · September 2011 Displaying guilt after a transgression serves to appease the victim and other group members, restore interpersonal relationships, and indicate the transgressors' awareness of and desire to conform to the group's norms. We investigated whether and when youn ... Full text Cite

Understanding of human communicative motives in domestic dogs

Journal Article Applied Animal Behaviour Science · September 1, 2011 Chimpanzees find it easier to locate food when a human prohibits them from going to a certain location than when she indicates that location helpfully. Human children, in contrast, use the cooperative gesture more readily. The question here was whether dom ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, share food in the same way after collaborative and individual food acquisition

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · September 1, 2011 We investigated the hypothesis that patterns of chimpanzee food sharing are influenced by whether individuals contributed to its acquisition collaboratively. In two experiments we exposed pairs of captive chimpanzees to food acquisition/sharing situations ... Full text Cite

Early social cognition in three cultural contexts.

Journal Article Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development · August 2011 The influence of culture on cognitive development is well established for school age and older children. But almost nothing is known about how different parenting and socialization practices in different cultures affect infants' and young children's earlie ... Full text Cite

Simple Mechanisms Can Explain Social Learning in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris)

Journal Article Ethology · August 1, 2011 Recent studies have suggested that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) engage in highly complex forms of social learning. Here, we critically assess the potential mechanisms underlying social learning in dogs using two problem-solving tasks. In a classical de ... Full text Cite

"Frequent frames" in German child-directed speech: a limited cue to grammatical categories.

Journal Article Cognitive science · August 2011 Mintz (2003) found that in English child-directed speech, frequently occurring frames formed by linking the preceding (A) and succeeding (B) word (A_x_B) could accurately predict the syntactic category of the intervening word (x). This has been successfull ... Full text Cite

Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees.

Journal Article Nature · July 2011 Humans actively share resources with one another to a much greater degree than do other great apes, and much human sharing is governed by social norms of fairness and equity. When in receipt of a windfall of resources, human children begin showing tendenci ... Full text Cite

German children's productivity with simple transitive and complement-clause constructions: Testing the effects of frequency and variability

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · May 1, 2011 The development of abstract schemas and productive rules in language is affected by both token and type frequencies. High token frequencies and surface similarities help to discover formal and functional commonalities between utterances and categorize them ... Full text Cite

How polish children switch from one case to another when using novel nouns: Challenges for models of inflectional morphology

Journal Article Language and Cognitive Processes · May 1, 2011 The two main models of children's acquisition of inflectional morphology-the Dual-Mechanism approach and the usage-based (schema-based) approach-have both been applied mainly to languages with fairly simple morphological systems. Here we report two studies ... Full text Cite

German children use prosody to identify participant roles in transitive sentences

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · May 1, 2011 Most studies examining children's understanding of transitive sentences focus on the morphosyntactic properties of the construction and ignore prosody. But adults use prosody in many different ways to interpret ambiguous sentences. In two studies we invest ... Full text Cite

Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language.

Journal Article Developmental science · May 2011 Young children interpret some acts performed by adults as normatively governed, that is, as capable of being performed either rightly or wrongly. In previous experiments, children have made this interpretation when adults introduced them to novel acts with ... Full text Cite

Social Engagement Leads 2-Year-Olds to Overestimate Others' Knowledge

Journal Article Infancy · May 1, 2011 Previous research has found that young children recognize an adult as being acquainted with an object most readily when the child and adult have previously engaged socially with that object together. In the current study, we tested the hypothesis that such ... Full text Cite

German-English-speaking children's mixed NPs with 'correct' agreement

Journal Article Bilingualism · April 1, 2011 Previous research has reported that bilingual children sometimes produce mixed noun phrases with 'correct' gender agreement- A s in der dog (der being a masculine determiner in German and the German word for dog, hund, being masculine as well). However, th ... Full text Cite

Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · March 2011 We investigated children's moral behaviour in situations in which a third party was harmed (the test case for possession of agent-neutral moral norms). A 3-year-old and two puppets each created a picture or clay sculpture, after which one puppet left the r ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees know that others make inferences.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · February 2011 If chimpanzees are faced with two opaque boards on a table, in the context of searching for a single piece of food, they do not choose the board lying flat (because if food was under there it would not be lying flat) but, rather, they choose the slanted on ... Full text Cite

Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational acts?

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2011 Range et al. (2007, Current Biology, 17, 868-872) found that dogs, Canis familiaris, copy others' means to achieve a goal more often when those means are the rational solution to a problem than when they are irrational. In our first experiment, we added a ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice when following a human pointing gesture.

Journal Article PloS one · January 2011 Domestic dogs are skillful at using the human pointing gesture. In this study we investigated whether dogs take contextual information into account when following pointing gestures, specifically, whether they follow human pointing gestures more readily in ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food items

Journal Article Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences · January 1, 2011 Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) sometimes help both humans and conspecifics in experimental situations in which immediate selfish benefits can be ruled out. However, in several experiments, chimpanzees have not provided food to a conspecific even when it wou ... Full text Cite

Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2011 Little is known about the origins of the pointing gesture. We sought to gain insight into its emergence by investigating individual differences in the pointing of 12-month-old infants in two ways. First, we looked at differences in the communicative and in ... Full text Cite

Different social motives in the gestural communication of chimpanzees and human children.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2011 Both chimpanzees and human infants use the pointing gesture with human adults, but it is not clear if they are doing so for the same social motives. In two studies, we presented chimpanzees and human 25-month-olds with the opportunity to point for a hidden ... Full text Cite

Young children share the spoils after collaboration

Journal Article Psychological Science · January 1, 2011 Egalitarian behavior is considered to be a species-typical component of human cooperation. Human adults tend to share resources equally, even if they have the opportunity to keep a larger portion for themselves. Recent experiments have suggested that this ... Full text Cite

Species differences in the rate of cognitive ontogeny among humans, chimpanzees, and bonobos

Conference AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY · January 1, 2011 Link to item Cite

Human Culture in Evolutionary Perspective

Chapter · December 24, 2010 Many animal species live in complex social groups, some of whom transmit information across generations "culturally". Humans' uniquely cultural way of life began with this kind of social organization but then acquired novel characteristics as a result of b ... Full text Cite

21-Month-olds understand the cooperative logic of requests

Journal Article Journal of Pragmatics · December 1, 2010 Human communication rests on a basic assumption of partner cooperativeness, including even requesting. In the current study, an adult made an ambiguous request for an object to 21-month-old infants, with one potential referent being right in front of her a ... Full text Cite

Young children selectively avoid helping people with harmful intentions.

Journal Article Child development · November 2010 Two studies investigated whether young children are selectively prosocial toward others, based on the others' moral behaviors. In Study 1 (N = 54), 3-year-olds watched 1 adult (the actor) harming or helping another adult. Children subsequently helped the h ... Full text Cite

Infants communicate in order to be understood.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2010 Infants intentionally communicate with others from before their 1st birthday. But there is some question about how they understand the communicative process. Do they understand that for their request to work the recipient must both understand the request a ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzee helping in collaborative and noncollaborative contexts

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · November 1, 2010 Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, help others in a variety of contexts. Some researchers have claimed that this only occurs when food is not involved and the recipient actively solicits help. In the current study, however, we found that chimpanzees often helpe ... Full text Cite

Bigger knows better: young children selectively learn rule games from adults rather than from peers.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · November 2010 Preschoolers' selective learning from adult versus peer models was investigated. Extending previous research, children from age 3 were shown to selectively learn simple rule games from adult rather than peer models. Furthermore, this selective learning was ... Full text Cite

Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds' attention to a contextually new referent

Journal Article Journal of Pragmatics · November 1, 2010 From the very beginning of language acquisition, young children are sensitive to what is given versus what is new in their discourse with others. Here we ask whether 24-month-olds use this skill to interpret prosodic highlighting as an invitation to focus ... Full text Cite

Infant cognition

Journal Article Current Biology · October 26, 2010 Until fairly recently, young infants were thought to be as cognitively incompetent as they were morally innocent. They were epistemological 'tabulae rasae', helpless 'bundles of reflexes' who spent all of their time sleeping, crying and sucking. In the fam ... Full text Cite

What Chimpanzees Know about Seeing, Revisited: An Explanation of the Third Kind

Chapter · September 1, 2010 Chimpanzees follow the gaze of conspecifics and humans -follow it past distractors and behind barriers, 'check back' with humans when gaze following does not yield interesting sights, use gestures appropriately depending on the visual access of their recip ... Full text Cite

Infants' use of shared experience in declarative pointing

Journal Article Infancy · September 1, 2010 In this study, we asked whether 14- and 18-month-old infants use the experiences they have previously shared with others when deciding what to point to for them declaratively. After sharing a particular type of referent with an adult in an excited manner, ... Full text Cite

Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children

Journal Article Evolution and Human Behavior · September 1, 2010 Humansw are the only primates that make music. But the evolutionary origins and functions of music are unclear. Given that in traditional cultures music making and dancing are often integral parts of important group ceremonies such as initiation rites, wed ... Full text Cite

Development ofword order in german complement-clause constructions: Effects of input frequencies, lexical items, and discourse function

Journal Article Language · September 1, 2010 We investigate the development of word order in German children's spontaneous production of complement clauses. From soon after their second birthday, young German children use both verb-final complements with complementizers and verb-second complements wi ... Full text Cite

Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees.

Journal Article PloS one · August 2010 While bonobos and chimpanzees are both genetically and behaviorally very similar, they also differ in significant ways. Bonobos are more cautious and socially tolerant while chimpanzees are more dependent on extractive foraging, which requires tools. The s ... Full text Open Access Cite

Language Development

Chapter · July 15, 2010 Full text Cite

Primate cognition.

Journal Article Topics in cognitive science · July 2010 As the cognitive revolution was slow to come to the study of animal behavior, the vast majority of what we know about primate cognition has been discovered in the last 30 years. Building on the recognition that the physical and social worlds of humans and ... Full text Cite

What's in a manner of speaking? Children's sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · July 2010 Do young children form "referential pacts"? If a person has referred to an object with a certain term (e.g., the horse), will children expect this person to use this term in the future but allow others to use a different expression (e.g., the pony)? One hu ... Full text Cite

Ape and human cognition: What's the difference?

Journal Article Current Directions in Psychological Science · June 25, 2010 Humans share the vast majority of their cognitive skills with other great apes. In addition, however, humans have also evolved a unique suite of cognitive skills and motivations-collectively referred to as shared intentionality-for living collaboratively, ... Full text Cite

36-month-olds conceal visual and auditory information from others.

Journal Article Developmental science · May 2010 By three years of age, children are skilled at assessing under which circumstances others can see things. However, nothing is known about whether they can use this knowledge to guide their own deceptive behaviour. Here we investigated 3-year-olds' ability ... Full text Cite

Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task.

Journal Article PloS one · May 2010 BackgroundIt is still unclear which observational learning mechanisms underlie the transmission of difficult problem-solving skills in chimpanzees. In particular, two different mechanisms have been proposed: imitation and emulation. Previous studi ... Full text Cite

Counterpoint commentary

Chapter · April 1, 2010 This chapter contains the authors' reactions to the previous chapters and stresses the similarities and differences between these theoretical views. It discusses that part of the debate concerns whether the first words are more like the indexical signs of ... Full text Cite

The Social Nature of Words and Word Learning

Chapter · April 1, 2010 This chapter describes research findings from the social-pragmatic approach. It discusses that Nameera Akhtar and Michael Tomasello's dramatic findings demonstrate how word learning occurs in some fairly complex, nonostensive situations amid the flow of so ... Full text Cite

Are Nouns Easier to Learn Than Verbs? Three Experimental Studies

Chapter · April 1, 2010 A current controversy in the study of word learning is whether it is conceptually easier to learn nouns as compared to verbs early in development. This chapter describes three experiments which address the noun-verb question in different ways. In the first ... Full text Cite

The influence of frequency and semantic similarity on how children learn grammar

Journal Article First Language · February 1, 2010 Lexically based learning and semantic analogy may both play a role in the learning of grammar. To investigate this, 5-year-old German children were trained on a miniature language (nominally English) involving two grammatical constructions, each of which w ... Full text Cite

On the Different Origins of Symbols and Grammar

Chapter · January 1, 2010 This chapter emphasises the role of psychology in language evolution, but claims that it was the separate evolution of capacities for using symbols and grammar (that is, syntactic structure) that distinguishes human communication from the communication of ... Full text Cite

The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture

Chapter · January 1, 2010 Human beings share many cognitive skills with their nearest primate relatives, especially those for dealing with the physical world of objects (and categories and quantities of objects) in space and their causal interrelations. But humans are, in addition, ... Full text Cite

Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2010 Adults refer young children's attention to things in two basic ways: through the use of pointing (and other deictic gestures) and words (and other linguistic conventions). In the current studies, we referred young children (2- and 4-year-olds) to things in ... Full text Cite

Studying extant species to model our past.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · January 2010 Full text Cite

Lexical frequency and exemplar-based learning effects in language acquisition: evidence from sentential complements

Journal Article Language Sciences · January 1, 2010 Usage-based approaches to language acquisition argue that children acquire the grammar of their target language using general-cognitive learning principles. The current paper reports on an experiment that tested a central assumption of the usage-based appr ... Full text Cite

The role of pronoun frames in early comprehension of transitive constructions in English

Journal Article Language Learning and Development · January 1, 2010 Case marking in English in available only on some pronouns and only in some cases. It is unknown whether young children acquiring English nevertheless make use of this highly restricted marking as a cue to sentence interpretation. The current study therefo ... Full text Cite

Young children's sensitivity to new and given information when answering predicate-focus questions

Journal Article Applied Psycholinguistics · January 1, 2010 In two studies we investigated 2-year-old children's answers to predicate-focus questions depending on the preceding context. Children were presented with a successive series of short video clips showing transitive actions (e.g., frog washing duck) in whic ... Full text Cite

Young children's comprehension of English SVO word order revisited: Testing the same children in act-out and intermodal preferential looking tasks

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2010 Act-out and intermodal preferential looking (IPL) tasks were administered to 67 English children aged 2-0, 2-9 and 3-5 to assess their comprehension of canonical SVO transitive word order with both familiar and novel verbs. Children at 3-5 and at 2-9 showe ... Full text Cite

Punishment

Chapter · January 1, 2010 Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be motivated by its effec ... Full text Cite

The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees.

Journal Article Psychological science · January 2010 Most studies of animal cognition focus on group performance and neglect individual differences and the correlational structure of cognitive abilities. Moreover, no previous studies have compared the correlational structure of cognitive abilities in nonhuma ... Full text Cite

Done wrong or said wrong? Young children understand the normative directions of fit of different speech acts.

Journal Article Cognition · November 2009 Young children use and comprehend different kinds of speech acts from the beginning of their communicative development. But it is not clear how they understand the conventional and normative structure of such speech acts. In particular, imperative speech a ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic signs.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2009 A key skill in early human development is the ability to comprehend communicative intentions as expressed in both nonlinguistic gestures and language. In the current studies, we confronted domestic dogs (some of whom knew many human 'words') with a task in ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees coordinate in a negotiation game

Journal Article Evolution and Human Behavior · November 1, 2009 A crucially important aspect of human cooperation is the ability to negotiate to cooperative outcomes when interests over resources conflict. Although chimpanzees and other social species may negotiate conflicting interests regarding travel direction or ac ... Full text Cite

Modeling children's early grammatical knowledge.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · October 2009 Theories of grammatical development differ in how much abstract knowledge they attribute to young children. Here, we report a series of experiments using a computational model to evaluate the explanatory power of child grammars based not on abstract rules ... Full text Cite

Universal grammar is dead

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · October 1, 2009 The idea of a biologically evolved, universal grammar with linguistic content is a myth, perpetuated by three spurious explanatory strategies of generative linguists. To make progress in understanding human linguistic competence, cognitive scientists must ... Full text Cite

Breed differences in domestic dogs' (Canis familiaris) comprehension of human communicative signals

Journal Article Interaction Studies · September 28, 2009 Recent research suggests that some human-like social skills evolved in dogs (Canis familiaris) during domestication as an incidental by-product of selection for "tame" forms of behavior. It is still possible, however, that the social skills of certain dog ... Full text Cite

Society need not be selfish

Journal Article Nature · September 3, 2009 Full text Cite

Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees.

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · September 2009 Recent empirical research has shed new light on the perennial question of human altruism. A number of recent studies suggest that from very early in ontogeny young children have a biological predisposition to help others achieve their goals, to share resou ... Full text Cite

Two-year-olds exclude novel objects as potential referents of novel words based on pragmatics.

Journal Article Cognition · September 2009 Many studies have established that children tend to exclude objects for which they already have a name as potential referents of novel words. In the current study we asked whether this exclusion can be triggered by social-pragmatic context alone without pr ... Full text Cite

Can chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) discriminate appearance from reality?

Journal Article Cognition · September 2009 A milestone in human development is coming to recognize that how something looks is not necessarily how it is. We tested appearance-reality understanding in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with a task requiring them to choose between a small grape and a big ... Full text Cite

Behavior. Like infant, like dog.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · September 2009 Full text Cite

Do great apes use emotional expressions to infer desires?

Journal Article Developmental science · September 2009 Although apes understand others' goals and perceptions, little is known about their understanding of others' emotional expressions. We conducted three studies following the general paradigm of Repacholi and colleagues (1997, 1998). In Study 1, a human reac ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of joint commitments.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · September 2009 When adults make a joint commitment to act together, they feel an obligation to their partner. In 2 studies, the authors investigated whether young children also understand joint commitments to act together. In the first study, when an adult orchestrated w ... Full text Cite

The roots of human altruism.

Journal Article British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953) · August 2009 Human infants as young as 14 to 18 months of age help others attain their goals, for example, by helping them to fetch out-of-reach objects or opening cabinets for them. They do this irrespective of any reward from adults (indeed external rewards undermine ... Full text Cite

The discourse bases of relativization: An investigation of young German and English-speaking children's comprehension of relative clauses

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · August 1, 2009 In numerous comprehension studies, across different languages, children have performed worse on object relatives (e.g., the dog that the cat chased) than on subject relatives (e.g., the dog that chased the cat). One possible reason for this is that the tes ... Full text Cite

Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture.

Journal Article Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences · August 2009 Some researchers have claimed that chimpanzee and human culture rest on homologous cognitive and learning mechanisms. While clearly there are some homologous mechanisms, we argue here that there are some different mechanisms at work as well. Chimpanzee cul ... Full text Cite

Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm.

Journal Article Cognition · August 2009 Recently, several studies have claimed that soon after their first birthday infants understand others' false beliefs. However, some have questioned these findings based on criticisms of the looking-time paradigms used. Here we report a new paradigm to test ... Full text Cite

'I want hold Postman Pat': An investigation into the acquisition of infinitival marker 'to'

Journal Article First Language · August 1, 2009 Infinitival-to omission errors (e.g., *I want hold Postman Pat) are produced by many English-speaking children early in development. This article aims to explain these omissions by investigating the emergence of infinitival-to, and its production/omission ... Full text Cite

Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist and usage-based accounts

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · August 1, 2009 This study tests accounts of co-reference errors whereby children allow "Mama Bear" and "her" to co-refer in sentences like "Mama Bear is washing her" (Chien and Wexler, Language Acquisition 1: 225-295, 1990). 63 children aged 4;6, 5;6 and 6;6 participated ... Full text Cite

Two-year-old children's production of multiword utterances: A usage-based analysis

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · August 1, 2009 Children generate novel utterances from the outset of multiword speech. In this study, we apply a usage-based method called 'traceback' to the multiword utterances of four two-year-olds to see how closely related these utterances are to their previous utte ... Full text Cite

A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes.

Journal Article Developmental science · July 2009 A nonverbal false belief task was administered to children (mean age 5 years) and two great ape species: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Because apes typically perform poorly in cooperative contexts, our task was competitive. Two ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective

Journal Article Behaviour · July 1, 2009 We investigated dogs' ability to take the visual perspective of humans. In the main study, each of two toys was placed on the dog's side of two small barriers (one opaque, one transparent). In experimental conditions, a human sat on the opposite side of th ... Full text Cite

Understanding of speaker certainty and false-belief reasoning: a comparison of Japanese and German preschoolers.

Journal Article Developmental science · July 2009 It has been repeatedly shown that when asked to identify a protagonist's false belief on the basis of his false statement, English-speaking 3-year-olds dismiss the statement and fail to attribute to him a false belief. In the present studies, we tested 3-y ... Full text Cite

Young children understand multiple pretend identities in their object play.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · June 2009 This set of studies examined the ability of 3-year-olds to conceptualize multiple pretend identities with objects. Rather than relying on verbal response measures, as has been done in the past, children's creative and inferential pretend actions were used ... Full text Cite

Young children's understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games.

Journal Article The British journal of developmental psychology · June 2009 We investigated young children's awareness of the context-relative rule structure of simple games. Two contexts were established in the form of spatial locations. Familiar objects were used in their conventional way at location 1, but acquired specific fun ... Full text Cite

Children's understanding of the agent-patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German, and English

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · May 1, 2009 Cantonese-, German-, and English-speaking children aged 2;6, 3,6, and 4,6 acted out transitive sentences containing novel verbs in three conditions: (1) agent and patient were cued redundantly by both word order and animacy; (2) agent and patient were mark ... Full text Cite

Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities.

Journal Article Psychological science · May 2009 One of the defining features of human language is displacement, the ability to make reference to absent entities. Here we show that prelinguistic, 12-month-old infants already can use a nonverbal pointing gesture to make reference to absent entities. We al ... Full text Cite

Normativity and context in young children's pretend play

Journal Article Cognitive Development · April 1, 2009 In two studies 3-year-olds' understanding of the context-specificity of normative rules was investigated through games of pretend play. In the first study, children protested against a character who joined a pretend game but treated the target object accor ... Full text Cite

Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · March 2009 In most research on the early ontogeny of sympathy, young children are presented with an overtly distressed person and their responses are observed. In the current study, the authors asked whether young children could also sympathize with a person to whom ... Full text Cite

Examining correlates of cooperation in autism: Imitation, joint attention, and understanding intentions.

Journal Article Autism : the international journal of research and practice · March 2009 The goal of the current study was to examine the contribution of three early social skills that may provide a foundation for cooperative performance in autism: (1) imitation, (2) joint attention, and (3) understanding of other people's intentions regarding ... Full text Cite

Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures.

Journal Article Developmental science · March 2009 We investigated whether 1-year-old infants use their shared experience with an adult to determine the meaning of a pointing gesture. In the first study, after two adults had each shared a different activity with the infant, one of the adults pointed to a t ... Full text Cite

Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children.

Journal Article Journal of experimental child psychology · March 2009 The human capacity to synchronize body movements to an external acoustic beat enables uniquely human behaviors such as music making and dancing. By hypothesis, these first evolved in human cultures as fundamentally social activities. We therefore hypothesi ... Full text Cite

Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm.

Journal Article American journal of primatology · February 2009 Recent studies have produced mixed evidence about inequity aversion in nonhuman primates. Brosnan et al. [Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 272:253-258, 2005] found inequity aversion in chimpanzees and argued that ef ... Full text Cite

Punishment

Chapter · January 1, 2009 Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be motivated by its effec ... Full text Cite

Flexibility in the semantics and syntax of children's early verb use.

Journal Article Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development · January 2009 Full text Cite

One-year-olds' understanding of nonverbal gestures directed to a third person

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2009 We investigated whether infants comprehend others' nonverbal communicative intentions directed to a third person, in an 'overhearing' context. An experimenter addressed an assistant and indicated a hidden toy's location by either gazing ostensively or poin ... Full text Cite

Young children's selective learning of rule games from reliable and unreliable models

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2009 We investigated preschoolers' selective learning from models that had previously appeared to be reliable or unreliable. Replicating previous research, children from 4 years selectively learned novel words from reliable over unreliable speakers. Extending p ... Full text Cite

Collective intentionality and cultural development

Journal Article Deutsche Zeitschrift fur Philosophie · December 1, 2008 Full text Cite

Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe.

Journal Article Cognition · November 2008 There is currently much controversy about which, if any, mental states chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates understand. In the current two studies we tested both chimpanzees' and human children's understanding of both knowledge-ignorance and false belie ... Full text Cite

Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2008 The current study investigated the influence of rewards on very young children's helping behavior. After 20-month-old infants received a material reward during a treatment phase, they subsequently were less likely to engage in further helping during a test ... Full text Cite

Assessing the validity of ape-human comparisons: a reply to Boesch (2007).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · November 2008 Boesch (2007) criticizes research comparing ape and human cognition on the basis of both internal and external validity. The authors show here that most of those criticisms are not valid because: (i) most threats to internal validity (e.g., conspecific exp ... Full text Cite

Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners.

Journal Article Cognition · September 2008 In the current study we investigated whether 12-month-old infants gesture appropriately for knowledgeable versus ignorant partners, in order to provide them with needed information. In two experiments we found that in response to a searching adult, 12-mont ... Full text Cite

Human behaviour: Share and share alike.

Journal Article Nature · August 2008 Full text Cite

Rapid learning of an abstract language-specific category: Polish children's acquisition of the instrumental construction.

Journal Article Journal of child language · August 2008 Rapid acquisition of linguistic categories or constructions is sometimes regarded as evidence of innate knowledge. In this paper, we examine Polish children's early understanding of an idiosyncratic, language-specific construction involving the instrumenta ... Full text Cite

Young German children's early syntactic competence: a preferential looking study.

Journal Article Developmental science · July 2008 Using a preferential looking methodology with novel verbs, Gertner, Fisher and Eisengart (2006) found that 21-month-old English children seemed to understand the syntactic marking of transitive word order in an abstract, verb-general way. In the current st ... Full text Cite

German children's comprehension of word order and case marking in causative sentences.

Journal Article Child development · July 2008 Two comprehension experiments were conducted to investigate whether German children are able to use the grammatical cues of word order and word endings (case markers) to identify agents and patients in a causative sentence and whether they weigh these two ... Full text Cite

An experimental study of nettle feeding in captive gorillas.

Journal Article American journal of primatology · June 2008 Mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) in Karisoke, Rwanda, feed on the stinging nettle Laportea alatipes by means of elaborate processing skills. Byrne [e.g. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences 3 ... Full text Cite

Automatic evaluation of syntactic learners in typologically-different languages

Journal Article Cognitive Systems Research · June 1, 2008 Human syntax acquisition involves a system that can learn constraints on possible word sequences in typologically-different human languages. Evaluation of computational syntax acquisition systems typically involves theory-specific or language-specific assu ... Full text Cite

Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes.

Journal Article Child development · May 2008 G. Gergely, H. Bekkering, and I. Király (2002) showed that 14-month-old infants imitate rationally, copying an adult's unusual action more often when it was freely chosen than when it was forced by some constraint. This suggests that infants understand oth ... Full text Cite

Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later.

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · May 2008 On the 30th anniversary of Premack and Woodruff's seminal paper asking whether chimpanzees have a theory of mind, we review recent evidence that suggests in many respects they do, whereas in other respects they might not. Specifically, there is solid evide ... Full text Cite

The sources of normativity: young children's awareness of the normative structure of games.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · May 2008 In two studies, the authors investigated 2- and 3-year-old children's awareness of the normative structure of conventional games. In the target conditions, an experimenter showed a child how to play a simple rule game. After the child and the experimenter ... Full text Cite

The acquisition of German relative clauses: a case study.

Journal Article Journal of child language · May 2008 This paper investigates the development of relative clauses in the speech of one German-speaking child aged 2 ; 0 to 5 ; 0. The earliest relative clauses we found in the data occur in topicalization constructions that are only a little different from simpl ... Full text Cite

Infants' visual and auditory communication when a partner is or is not visually attending.

Journal Article Infant behavior & development · April 2008 In the current study we investigated infants' communication in the visual and auditory modalities as a function of the recipient's visual attention. We elicited pointing at interesting events from thirty-two 12-month olds and thirty-two 18-month olds in tw ... Full text Cite

The early ontogeny of human-dog communication

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · March 1, 2008 Although dogs, Canis familiaris, are skilful at responding to human social cues, the role of ontogeny in the development of these abilities has not been systematically examined. We studied the ability of very young dog puppies to follow human communicative ... Full text Cite

Cultural Learning and Learning Culture

Chapter · February 27, 2008 The universals and cultural variations of human development have been the focus of fruitful study by anthropologists for decades. In recent years psychologists also have directed their attention, long overdue, to understanding development in cultural conte ... Full text Cite

Cognitive Linguistics

Chapter · February 26, 2008 A central goal of cognitive science is to understand how human beings comprehend, produce, and acquire natural languages. Throughout the brief history of modern cognitive science, the linguistic theory that has been most prominent in this endeavor is gener ... Full text Cite

Helping and cooperation in children with autism.

Journal Article Journal of autism and developmental disorders · February 2008 Helping and cooperation are central to human social life. Here, we report two studies investigating these social behaviors in children with autism and children with developmental delay. In the first study, both groups of children helped the experimenter at ... Full text Cite

Response [6]

Journal Article Science · January 18, 2008 Cite

Grammar

Journal Article · January 1, 2008 To acquire competence with a natural language, young children must master the grammatical constructions of their language(s). In this article we outline the main theoretical issues in the field and trace the developmental path children follow from talking ... Full text Cite

Cultural transmission: A view from chimpanzees and human infants

Chapter · January 1, 2008 Introduction Primates are highly social beings. They begin their lives clinging to their mother and nursing, and they spend their next few months, or even years, still in proximity to her. Adult primates live in close-knit social groups, for the most part, ... Full text Cite

Graded representations in the acquisition of English and German transitive constructions

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2008 English and German children aged 2 years 4 months and 4 years heard both novel and familiar verbs in sentences whose form was grammatical, but which mismatched the event they were watching (e.g., 'The frog is pushing the lion', when the lion was actually t ... Full text Cite

Behavioral cues that great apes use to forage for hidden food.

Journal Article Animal cognition · January 2008 We conducted three studies to examine whether the four great ape species (chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas, and orangutans) are able to use behavioral experimenter-given cues in an object-choice task. In the subsequent experimental conditions subjects were p ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees do not take into account what others can hear in a competitive situation.

Journal Article Animal cognition · January 2008 Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) know what others can and cannot see in a competitive situation. Does this reflect a general understanding the perceptions of others? In a study by Hare et al. (2000) pairs of chimpanzees competed over two pieces of food. Subor ... Full text Cite

Fourteen-month-olds know what "we" have shared in a special way

Journal Article Infancy · January 1, 2008 People often express excitement to each other when encountering an object that they have shared together previously in some special way. This study investigated whether 14-month-old infants know precisely what they have and have not shared in a special way ... Full text Cite

The development of the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs.

Journal Article Journal of deaf studies and deaf education · January 2008 Early developmental psychologists viewed iconic representation as cognitively less complex than other forms of symbolic thought. It is therefore surprising that iconic signs are not acquired more easily than arbitrary signs by young language learners. One ... Full text Cite

CHILDREN’S FIRST LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FROM A USAGE-BASED PERSPECTIVE1

Chapter · January 1, 2008 There are, however, major debates as to what they bring to this language learning: do they come with innate, specifically syntactic skills or, rather, with more general cognitive and interactive skills? In this chapter, we will argue for the latter and sug ... Full text Cite

Grammar

Chapter · January 1, 2008 To acquire competence with a natural language, young children must master the grammatical constructions of their language(s). In this article we outline the main theoretical issues in the field and trace the developmental path children follow from talking ... Full text Cite

Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours?

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2008 Reciprocal interactions observed in animals may persist because individuals keep careful account of services exchanged with each group member. To test whether chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, possess the cognitive skills required for this type of contingency- ... Full text Cite

Fourteen-month-olds know what others experience only in joint engagement.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2007 We investigated how 14-month-old infants know what others know. In two studies, an infant played with each of two objects in turn while an experimenter was present. Then the experimenter left the room, and the infant played with a third object with an assi ... Full text Cite

How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: a training study.

Journal Article Child development · November 2007 This training study investigates how children learn to refer to things unambiguously. Two hundred twenty-four children aged 2.6, 3.6, and 4.6 years were pre- and posttested for their ability to request stickers from a dense array. Between test sessions, ch ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation.

Journal Article Animal cognition · October 2007 Chimpanzee's perspective-taking abilities are currently disputed. Here we show that in some food competition contexts, subordinate chimpanzees do take the visual perspective of dominant individuals, preferentially targeting a hidden piece of the food that ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an ultimatum game.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · October 2007 Traditional models of economic decision-making assume that people are self-interested rational maximizers. Empirical research has demonstrated, however, that people will take into account the interests of others and are sensitive to norms of cooperation an ... Full text Cite

Object relatives made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children's processing of relative clauses

Journal Article Language and Cognitive Processes · September 1, 2007 We present the results from four studies, two corpora and two experimental, which suggest that English- and German-speaking children (3;1-4;9 years) use multiple constraints to process and produce object relative clauses. Our two corpora studies show that ... Full text Cite

Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · September 2007 Humans have many cognitive skills not possessed by their nearest primate relatives. The cultural intelligence hypothesis argues that this is mainly due to a species-specific set of social-cognitive skills, emerging early in ontogeny, for participating and ... Full text Cite

Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words.

Journal Article Journal of child language · August 2007 German children aged 2;1 heard a sentence containing a nonce noun and a nonce verb (Der Feks miekt). Either the noun or the verb was prosodically highlighted by increased pitch, duration and loudness. Independently, either the object or the action in the o ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful.

Journal Article Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America · August 2007 People are willing to punish others at a personal cost, and this apparently antisocial tendency can stabilize cooperation. What motivates humans to punish noncooperators is likely a combination of aversion to both unfair outcomes and unfair intentions. Her ... Full text Cite

Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally.

Journal Article Developmental science · July 2007 Human infants imitate others' actions 'rationally': they copy a demonstrator's action when that action is freely chosen, but less when it is forced by some constraint (Gergely, Bekkering & Király, 2002). We investigated whether enculturated chimpanzees (Pa ... Full text Cite

Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children.

Journal Article PLoS biology · July 2007 People often act on behalf of others. They do so without immediate personal gain, at cost to themselves, and even toward unfamiliar individuals. Many researchers have claimed that such altruism emanates from a species-unique psychology not found in humans' ... Full text Cite

Great apes' understanding of other individuals' line of sight.

Journal Article Psychological science · May 2007 Previous research has shown that many social animals follow the gaze of other individuals. However, knowledge about how this skill differs between species and whether it shows a relationship with genetic distance from humans is still fragmentary. In the pr ... Full text Cite

French children's use and correction of weird word orders: a constructivist account.

Journal Article Journal of child language · May 2007 Using the weird word order methodology (Akhtar, 1999), we investigated children's understanding of SVO word order in French, a language with less consistent argument ordering patterns than English. One hundred and twelve French children (ages 2; 10 and 3; ... Full text Cite

A new look at infant pointing.

Journal Article Child development · May 2007 The current article proposes a new theory of infant pointing involving multiple layers of intentionality and shared intentionality. In the context of this theory, evidence is presented for a rich interpretation of prelinguistic communication, that is, one ... Full text Cite

Understanding of intentions, shared intentions: The origins of cultural thinking

Journal Article Magyar Pszichologiai Szemle · April 1, 2007 We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities require ... Full text Cite

Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis.

Conference Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences · April 2007 Nicholas Humphrey's social intelligence hypothesis proposed that the major engine of primate cognitive evolution was social competition. Lev Vygotsky also emphasized the social dimension of intelligence, but he focused on human primates and cultural things ... Full text Cite

Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age.

Journal Article Developmental science · March 2007 There is currently controversy over the nature of 1-year-olds' social-cognitive understanding and motives. In this study we investigated whether 12-month-old infants point for others with an understanding of their knowledge states and with a prosocial moti ... Full text Cite

How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · March 2007 Fourteen- and 18-month-old infants observed an adult experiencing each of 2 objects (experienced objects) and then leaving the room; the infant then played with a 3rd object while the adult was gone (unexperienced object). The adult interacted with the 2 e ... Full text Cite

Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: the cooperative eye hypothesis.

Journal Article Journal of human evolution · March 2007 As compared with other primates, humans have especially visible eyes (e.g., white sclera). One hypothesis is that this feature of human eyes evolved to make it easier for conspecifics to follow an individual's gaze direction in close-range joint attentiona ... Full text Cite

Reference and attitude in infant pointing.

Journal Article Journal of child language · February 2007 We investigated two main components of infant declarative pointing, reference and attitude, in two experiments with a total of 106 preverbal infants at 1;0. When an experimenter (E) responded to the declarative pointing of these infants by attending to an ... Full text Cite

"This way!", "No! That way!"-3-year olds know that two people can have mutually incompatible desires

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2007 In theory of mind research, there is a long standing dispute about whether children come to understand the subjectivity of both desires and beliefs at the same time (around age 4), or whether there is an asymmetry such that desires are understood earlier. ... Full text Cite

Shared intentionality.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2007 We argue for the importance of processes of shared intentionality in children's early cognitive development. We look briefly at four important social-cognitive skills and how they are transformed by shared intentionality. In each case, we look first at a k ... Full text Cite

Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age

Journal Article Infancy · January 1, 2007 Two experiments investigated the proclivity of 14-month-old infants (a) to altruistically help others toward individual goals, and (b) to cooperate toward a shared goal. The infants helped another person by handing over objects the other person was unsucce ... Full text Cite

Are apes really inequity averse?

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · December 2006 Brosnan et al. (Brosnan, S. F. Schiff, H. C. & de Waal, F. B. M. 2005 Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B272, 253-258) found that chimpanzees showed increased levels of rejection for less-preferred food ... Full text Cite

Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants

Journal Article Infancy · December 1, 2006 Gergely, Bekkering, and Király (2002) demonstrated that 14-month-old infants engage in "rational imitation." To investigate the development and flexibility of this skill, we tested 12-month-olds on a different but analogous task. Infants watched as an adul ... Full text Cite

Push or pull: Imitation vs. emulation in great apes and human children

Journal Article Ethology · December 1, 2006 All four species of great apes and young human children (12-24 mo of age) were administered an imitation task designed to distinguish between results learning (emulation) and action learning (imitation). Some subjects were exposed to a demonstrator either ... Full text Cite

Two-year-olds grasp the intentional structure of pretense acts.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2006 Twenty-two- and 27-month-old children were tested for their understanding of pretending as a specific intentional action form. Pairs of superficially similar behaviors - pretending to perform an action and trying to perform that action - were demonstrated ... Full text Cite

Goats' behaviour in a competitive food paradigm: Evidence for perspective taking?

Journal Article Behaviour · November 1, 2006 Many mammalian species are highly social, creating intra-group competition for such things as food and mates. Recent research with nonhuman primates indicates that in competitive situations individuals know what other individuals can and cannot see, and th ... Full text Cite

Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition

Journal Article Linguistic Review · October 1, 2006 The early phases of syntactic acquisition are characterized by many input frequency and item effects, which argue against theories assuming innate access to classical syntactic categories. In formulating an alternative view, we consider both prototype and ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding.

Journal Article Cognition · October 2006 There is little experimental evidence that any non-human species is capable of purposefully attempting to manipulate the psychological states of others deceptively (e.g., manipulating what another sees). We show here that chimpanzees, one of humans' two cl ... Full text Cite

Infants determine others' focus of attention by pragmatics and exlusion

Journal Article Journal of Cognition and Development · September 15, 2006 In the studies presented here, infants' understanding of others' attention was assessed when gaze direction cues were not diagnostic. Fourteen-, 18- and 24-month-olds witnessed an adult look to the side of an object and express excitement. In 1 experimenta ... Full text Cite

Level I perspective-taking at 24 months of age

Journal Article British Journal of Developmental Psychology · September 11, 2006 The current study sought to determine the age at which children first engage in Level I visual perspective-taking, in which they understand that the content of what another person sees in a situation may sometimes differ from what they see. An adult entere ... Full text Cite

Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation.

Journal Article Developmental science · September 2006 Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) (Study 1) and 18- and 24-month-old human children (Study 2) participated in a novel communicative task. A human experimenter (E) hid food or a toy in one of two opaque containers before gesturing tow ... Full text Cite

Comparing different accounts of inversion errors in children's non-subject wh-questions: 'What experimental data can tell us?'.

Journal Article Journal of child language · August 2006 This study investigated different accounts of children's acquisition of non-subject wh-questions. Questions using each of 4 wh-words (what, who, how and why), and 3 auxiliaries (BE, DO and CAN) in 3sg and 3pl form were elicited from 28 children aged 3;6-4; ... Full text Cite

Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance constraints on cooperation

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · August 1, 2006 The cooperative abilities of captive chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, in experiments do not match the sophistication that might be predicted based on their naturally occurring cooperative behaviours. This discrepancy might partly be because in previous experi ... Full text Cite

12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others

Journal Article Journal of Cognition and Development · July 25, 2006 Classically, infants are thought to point for 2 main reasons: (a) They point imperatively when they want an adult to do something for them (e.g., give them something; "Juice!"), and (b) they point declaratively when they want an adult to share attention wi ... Full text Cite

The role of experience and discourse in children's developing understanding of pretend play actions

Journal Article British Journal of Developmental Psychology · June 15, 2006 The present work investigated the development of an explicit understanding of pretend play actions. Study I revealed a long décalage between earlier implicit understanding of pretence as an intentional activity and a later more explicit understanding. Stud ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory information from others.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · May 2006 Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) competed with a human for food. The human sat inside a booth, with 1 piece of food to her left and 1 to her right, which she could retract from her chimpanzee competitor's reach as needed. In Experiment 1, chimpanzees could ap ... Full text Cite

Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees.

Journal Article Child development · May 2006 Human children 18-24 months of age and 3 young chimpanzees interacted in 4 cooperative activities with a human adult partner. The human children successfully participated in cooperative problem-solving activities and social games, whereas the chimpanzees w ... Full text Cite

Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task.

Journal Article Animal cognition · April 2006 In previous studies great apes have shown little ability to locate hidden food using a physical marker placed by a human directly on the target location. In this study, we hypothesized that the perceptual similarity between an iconic cue and the hidden rew ... Full text Cite

The distributed learning effect for children's acquisition of an abstract syntactic construction

Journal Article Cognitive Development · April 1, 2006 In many cognitive domains, learning is more effective when exemplars are distributed over a number of sessions than when they are all presented within one session. The present study investigated this distributed learning effect with respect to English-spea ... Full text Cite

Examining the role of lexical frequency in the acquisition and processing of sentential complements

Journal Article Cognitive Development · April 1, 2006 We present empirical data showing that the relative frequency with which a verb normally appears in a syntactic construction predicts young children's ability to remember and repeat sentences instantiating that construction. Children aged 2;10-5;8 years we ... Full text Cite

What's in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees.

Journal Article Proceedings. Biological sciences · April 2006 Sensitivity to fairness may influence whether individuals choose to engage in acts that are mutually beneficial, selfish, altruistic, or spiteful. In a series of three experiments, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) could pull a rope to access out-of-reach food ... Full text Cite

Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · March 2006 Human beings routinely help others to achieve their goals, even when the helper receives no immediate benefit and the person helped is a stranger. Such altruistic behaviors (toward non-kin) are extremely rare evolutionarily, with some theorists even propos ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · March 2006 Humans collaborate with non-kin in special ways, but the evolutionary foundations of these collaborative skills remain unclear. We presented chimpanzees with collaboration problems in which they had to decide when to recruit a partner and which potential p ... Full text Cite

Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · February 2006 Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and great apes from the genus Pan were tested on a series of object choice tasks. In each task, the location of hidden food was indicated for subjects by some kind of communicative, behavioral, or physical cue. On the basis ... Full text Cite

The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children's use of referring expressions

Journal Article Applied Psycholinguistics · January 1, 2006 Choosing appropriate referring expressions requires assessing whether a referent is "available" to the addressee either perceptually or through discourse. In Study 1, we found that 3- and 4-year-olds, but not 2-year-olds, chose different referring expressi ... Full text Cite

Gestural communication of orangutans (pongo pygmaeus)

Journal Article Gesture · January 1, 2006 This study represents a systematic investigation of the communicative repertoire of Sumatran orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii), with a focus on intentional signals in two groups of captive orangutans. The goal was to analyze the signal repertoire with res ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food.

Journal Article Animal cognition · January 2006 Dogs can use the placement of an arbitrary marker to locate hidden food in an object-choice situation. We tested domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in three studies aimed at pinning down the relative contributions of the human's hand and the marker itself. W ... Full text Cite

Beyond formalities: The case of language acquisition

Journal Article Linguistic Review · December 12, 2005 Generative grammar retained from American structural linguistics the 'formal' approach, which basically effaces the semantic and pragmatic dimensions of grammar. This creates serious problems for an account of language acquisition, most especially the prob ... Full text Cite

German-speaking children's productivity with syntactic constructions and case morphology: Local cues act locally

Journal Article First Language · December 1, 2005 It has been proposed that children acquiring case-marking languages might be quicker to acquire certain constructions than children acquiring word order languages, because the cues involved in grammatical morphology are more 'local', whereas word order is ... Full text Cite

A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses

Journal Article Language · December 1, 2005 This study reconsiders the acquisition of relative clauses based on data from two sentence-repetition tasks. Using materials modeled on the relative constructions of spontaneous child speech, we asked four-year-old English- and German-speaking children to ... Full text Cite

Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism

Journal Article Infancy · December 1, 2005 Three types of role reversal imitation were investigated in typically developing 12-and 18-month-old infants and in children with autism and other developmental delays. Many typically developing infants at both ages engaged in each of the 2 types of dyadic ... Full text Cite

Verb learning in children with SLI: frequency and spacing effects.

Journal Article Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR · December 2005 PurposeThis study explored the effect of frequency (number of presentations), and spacing (period between presentations) on verb learning in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Children learn words more efficiently when presentations ... Full text Cite

One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game.

Journal Article Developmental science · November 2005 This study explored infants' ability to infer communicative intent as expressed in non-linguistic gestures. Sixty children aged 14, 18 and 24 months participated. In the context of a hiding game, an adult indicated for the child the location of a hidden to ... Full text Cite

Young children's sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions

Journal Article Applied Psycholinguistics · October 1, 2005 Speakers use different types of referring expressions depending on what the listener knows or is attending to; for example, they use pronouns for objects that are already present in the immediate discourse or perceptual context. In a first study we found t ... Full text Cite

Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition.

Journal Article The Behavioral and brain sciences · October 2005 We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities require ... Full text Cite

In search of the uniquely human

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · October 1, 2005 As Bruner so eloquently points out, and Gauvain echoes, human beings are unique in their "locality." Individual groups of humans develop their own unique ways of symbolizing and doing things - and these can be very different from the ways of other groups, ... Full text Cite

Human-like social skills in dogs?

Journal Article Trends in cognitive sciences · September 2005 Domestic dogs are unusually skilled at reading human social and communicative behavior--even more so than our nearest primate relatives. For example, they use human social and communicative behavior (e.g. a pointing gesture) to find hidden food, and they k ... Full text Cite

Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens).

Journal Article Animal cognition · July 2005 There is currently much debate about the nature of social learning in chimpanzees. The main question is whether they can copy others' actions, as opposed to reproducing the environmental effects of these actions using their own preexisting behavioral strat ... Full text Cite

Young children's knowledge of the "determiner" and "adjective" categories.

Journal Article Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR · June 2005 Children's understanding of the grammatical categories of "determiner" and "adjective" was examined using 2 different methodologies. In Experiment 1, children heard novel nouns combined with either a or the. Few 2-year-olds, but nearly all 3- and 4-year-ol ... Full text Cite

All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · May 2005 Following the gaze direction of conspecifics is an adaptive skill that enables individuals to obtain useful information about the location of food, predators, and group mates. In the current study, the authors compared the gaze-following skills of all 4 gr ... Full text Cite

Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of intentional action.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · March 2005 Infants experienced a female adult handling them toys. Sometimes, however, the transaction failed, either because the adult was in various ways unwilling to give the toy (e.g., she teased the child with it or played with it herself) or else because she was ... Full text Cite

Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2005 Gaze following is a basic social cognitive skill with many potential benefits for animals that live in social groups. At least five primate species are known to follow the gaze of conspecifics, but there have been no studies on gaze following in other mamm ... Full text Cite

The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2005 Akhtar [Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child Language, 26, 339-356] taught children novel verbs in ungrammatical word orders. Her results suggested that the acquisition of ... Full text Cite

The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees.

Journal Article Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development · January 2005 We report a series of 10 studies on the social-cognitive abilities of three young chimpanzees. The studies were all ones previously conducted with human infants. The chimpanzees were 1-5 years of age, had been raised mostly by humans, and were tested mostl ... Full text Cite

The gestural communication of apes

Journal Article Gesture · January 1, 2005 Gestural communication of nonhuman primates may allow insight into the evolutionary scenario of human communication given the flexible use and learning of gestures as opposed to vocalizations. This paper provides an overview of the work on the gestural com ... Full text Cite

Particle placement in early child language: A multifactorial analysis

Journal Article Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory · January 1, 2005 Recent studies of the English verb particle construction have shown that particle placement varies with a variety of linguistic features, which seem to influence the speaker's choice of a particular position. The current study investigates whether children ... Full text Cite

Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2005 In the context of an imitation game, 12- and 18-month-old infants saw an adult do such things as make a toy mouse hop across a mat (with sound effects). In one condition (House), the adult ended by placing the mouse in a toy house, whereas in another condi ... Full text Cite

On tools and toys: how children learn to act on and pretend with 'virgin objects'.

Journal Article Developmental science · January 2005 The focus of the present study was the role of cultural learning in infants' acquisition of pretense actions with objects. In three studies, 18- and 24-month-olds (n = 64) were presented with novel objects, and either pretense or instrumental actions were ... Full text Cite

Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): repertoire and use.

Journal Article American journal of primatology · January 2005 This article aims to provide an inventory of the communicative gestures used by bonobos (Pan paniscus), based on observations of subadult bonobos and descriptions of gestural signals and similar behaviors in wild and captive bonobo groups. In addition, we ... Full text Cite

The changing role of iconicity in non-verbal symbol learning: A U-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures

Journal Article Journal of Cognition and Development · December 1, 2004 This article reports 2 experiments examining the changing role of iconicity in symbol learning and its implications regarding the mechanisms supporting symbol-to-referent mapping. Experiment 1 compared 18- and 26-month-olds' mapping of iconic gestures (e.g ... Full text Cite

Two hypotheses about primate cognition

Journal Article Deutsche Zeitschrift fur Philosophie · December 1, 2004 Cite

Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees.

Journal Article American journal of primatology · December 2004 Gestural communication in a group of 19 captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) was observed, with particular attention paid to gesture sequences (combinations). A complete inventory of gesture sequences is reported. The majority of these sequences were repe ... Full text Cite

A dense corpus study of past tense and plural overregularization in English.

Journal Article Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR · December 2004 In the "blocking-and-retrieval-failure" account of overregularization (OR; G. F. Marcus, 1995; G. F. Marcus et al., 1992), the claim that a symbolic rule generates regular inflection is founded on pervasively low past tense OR rates and the lack of a subst ... Full text Cite

Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers

Journal Article Applied Animal Behaviour Science · October 1, 2004 Previous studies have shown that dogs have developed a special sensitivity to the communicative signals and attentional states of humans. The aim of the current study was to further investigate what dogs know about the visual perception of humans and thems ... Full text Cite

Body orientation and face orientation: two factors controlling apes' behavior from humans.

Journal Article Animal cognition · October 2004 A number of animal species have evolved the cognitive ability to detect when they are being watched by other individuals. Precisely what kind of information they use to make this determination is unknown. There is particular controversy in the case of the ... Full text Cite

Syntax or semantics? Response to Lidz et al.

Journal Article Cognition · September 2004 Full text Cite

'Unwilling' versus 'unable': chimpanzees' understanding of human intentional action.

Journal Article Developmental science · September 2004 Understanding the intentional actions of others is a fundamental part of human social cognition and behavior. An important question is therefore whether other animal species, especially our nearest relatives the chimpanzees, also understand the intentional ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · September 1, 2004 In a series of four experiments, chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, were given two cognitive tasks, an object choice task and a discrimination task (based on location), each in the context of either cooperation or competition. In both tasks chimpanzees performe ... Full text Cite

Introduction

Book · August 22, 2004 Full text Cite

Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest.

Journal Article Developmental science · June 2004 Infants point for various motives. Classically, one such motive is declarative, to share attention and interest with adults to events. Recently, some researchers have questioned whether infants have this motivation. In the current study, an adult reacted t ... Full text Cite

The pragmatics of primate communication

Journal Article Psychologie Francaise · June 1, 2004 Pragmatics is about how individuals use their inventory of semiotic devices, the strategic choices they make, in particular acts of communication. An interesting question is the degree to which other animal species, especially our nearest primate relatives ... Full text Cite

Young children know that trying is not pretending: a test of the "behaving-as-if" construal of children's early concept of pretense.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · May 2004 In 3 studies, young children were tested for their understanding of pretend actions. In Studies 1 and 2, pairs of superficially similar behaviors were presented to 26- and 36-month-old children in an imitation game. In one case the behavior was marked as t ... Full text Cite

Training 2;6-year-olds to produce the transitive construction: the role of frequency, semantic similarity and shared syntactic distribution.

Journal Article Developmental science · February 2004 Childers and Tomasello (2001) found that training 2 1/2-year-olds on the English transitive construction greatly improves their performance on a post-test in which they must use novel verbs in that construction. In the current study, we replicated Childers ... Full text Cite

12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers.

Journal Article Developmental science · February 2004 Infants follow the gaze direction of others from the middle of the first year of life. In attempting to determine how infants understand the looking behavior of adults, a number of recent studies have blocked the adult's line of sight in some way (e.g. wit ... Full text Cite

Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough?

Journal Article Journal of Child Language · January 1, 2004 There has been relatively little discussion in the field of child language acquisition about how best to sample from children's spontaneous speech, particularly with regard to quantitative issues. Here we provide quantitative information designed to help r ... Full text Cite

To move or not to move: How apes adjust to the attentional state of others

Journal Article Interaction Studies · January 1, 2004 A previous observational study suggested that when faced with a partner with its back turned, chimpanzees tend to move around to the front of a non-attending partner and then gesture-rather than gesturing once to attract attention and then again to convey ... Full text Cite

Learning through others

Journal Article Daedalus · January 1, 2004 Full text Cite

Social communication in siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus): use of gestures and facial expressions.

Journal Article Primates; journal of primatology · January 2004 The current study represents the first systematic investigation of the social communication of captive siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus). The focus was on intentional signals, including tactile and visual gestures, as well as facial expressions and actio ... Full text Cite

Testing the abstractness of children's linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children

Journal Article Developmental Science · November 1, 2003 The current studies used a priming methodology to assess the abstractness of children's early syntactic constructions. In the main study, 3-, 4- and 6-year-old children were asked to describe a prime picture by repeating either an active or a passive sente ... Full text Cite

Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · September 2003 Infants at 12 and 18 months of age played with 2 adults and 2 new toys. For a 3rd toy, however, 1 of the adults left the room while the child and the other adult played with it. This adult then returned, looked at all 3 toys aligned on a tray, showed great ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans.

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · September 2003 Twelve domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) were given a series of trials in which they were forbidden to take a piece of visible food. In some trials, the human continued to look at the dog throughout the trial (control condition), whereas in others, the huma ... Full text Cite

The role of the input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English.

Journal Article Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR · August 2003 During the early stages of language acquisition, children pass through a stage of development when they produce both finite and nonfinite verb forms in finite contexts (e.g., "it go there," "it goes there"). Theorists who assume that children operate with ... Full text Cite

What paradox? A response to Naigles (2002).

Journal Article Cognition · July 2003 Full text Cite

The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: a training study.

Journal Article Child development · July 2003 The current study used a training methodology to determine whether different kinds of linguistic interaction play a causal role in children's development of false belief understanding. After 3 training sessions, 3-year-old children improved their false bel ... Full text Cite

Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): gestural repertoire, learning, and use.

Journal Article American journal of primatology · July 2003 In the present study we investigated the gestural communication of gorillas (Gorilla gorilla). The subjects were 13 gorillas (1-6 years old) living in two different groups in captivity. Our goal was to compile the gestural repertoire of subadult gorillas, ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees versus humans: It's not that simple

Journal Article Trends in Cognitive Sciences · June 1, 2003 Full text Cite

Early syntactic creativity: a usage-based approach.

Journal Article Journal of child language · May 2003 The aim of the current study was to determine the degree to which a sample of one child's creative utterances related to utterances that the child previously produced. The utterances to be accounted for were all of the intelligible, multi-word utterances p ... Full text Cite

Children extend both words and non-verbal actions to novel exemplars

Journal Article Developmental Science · April 1, 2003 Markson and Bloom (1997) found that some learning processes involved in children's acquisition of a new word are also involved in their acquisition of a new fact. They argued that these findings provided evidence against a domain-specific system for word l ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees understand psychological states - The question is which ones and to what extent

Journal Article Trends in Cognitive Sciences · April 1, 2003 New data suggest that relatively drastic revisions are needed in our theoretical accounts of what other animal species understand about the psychological states of others. Specifically, chimpanzees seem to understand some things about what others do and do ... Full text Cite

Language and social understanding: Commentary on Nelson et al.

Journal Article Human Development · February 3, 2003 Full text Cite

What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality

Journal Article Mind and Language · January 1, 2003 It is widely believed that what distinguishes the social cognition of humans from that of other animals is the belief-desire psychology of four-year-old children and adults (so-called theory of mind). We argue here that this is actually the second ontogene ... Full text Cite

A construction based analysis of child directed speech

Journal Article Cognitive Science · January 1, 2003 The child directed speech of twelve English-speaking mothers was analyzed in terms of utterance-level constructions. First, the mothers' utterances were categorized in terms of general constructional categories such as Wh-questions, copulas and transitives ... Full text Cite

Do capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, know what conspecifics do and do not see?

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2003 Capuchin monkeys were tested in five experiments in which two individuals competed over food. When given a choice between retrieving a piece of food that was visible or hidden from the dominant, subordinate animals preferred to retrieve hidden food. This p ... Full text Cite

Things are what they do: Katherine Nelson's functional approach to language and cognition

Journal Article Journal of Cognition and Development · December 1, 2002 This article attempts to summarize Katherine Nelson's theoretical and empirical contributions to the ontogenetic study of language and cognition. Nelson's approach has consistently emphasized the function of language and linguistic concepts in children's l ... Full text Cite

Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional actions from massed or distributed exposures.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2002 Two-year-old children were taught either 6 novel nouns, 6 novel verbs, or 6 novel actions over 1 month. In each condition, children were exposed to some items in massed presentations (on a single day) and some in distributed presentations (over the 2 weeks ... Full text Cite

The domestication of social cognition in dogs.

Journal Article Science (New York, N.Y.) · November 2002 Dogs are more skillful than great apes at a number of tasks in which they must read human communicative signals indicating the location of hidden food. In this study, we found that wolves who were raised by humans do not show these same skills, whereas dom ... Full text Cite

Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task.

Journal Article Child development · September 2002 This study investigated children's understanding of others' intentions in a social learning context. Specifically, it investigated whether knowing an adult's prior intention before the adult gives a demonstration influences what children learn from the dem ... Full text Cite

A new false belief test for 36-month-olds

Journal Article British Journal of Developmental Psychology · September 1, 2002 We report two studies that suggest that some 36-month-old (and younger) children understand others' false beliefs. In the false belief conditions, children and two adults (E1 and E2) watched as an object was put into a container. E1 left the room, and E2 s ... Full text Cite

German children's productivity with tense morphology: the Perfekt (present perfect).

Journal Article Journal of child language · August 2002 Two nonce-word studies examined German-speaking children's productivity with the Perfekt (present perfect) from 2;6 to 3;6. The German Perfekt consists of the past participle of the main verb and an inflected form of an auxiliary (either haben 'have' or se ... Full text Cite

A tale of two theories: response to Fisher.

Journal Article Cognition · March 2002 Full text Cite

The role of pronouns in young children's acquisition of the English transitive construction.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 2001 Two studies investigating the linguistic representations underlying English-speaking 2 1/2-year-olds' production of transitive utterances are reported. The first study was a training study in which half the children heard utterances with full nouns as agen ... Full text Cite

The acquisition of English dative constructions

Journal Article Applied Psycholinguistics · June 1, 2001 We analyzed the three main types of English dative constructions - the double-object dative, the to dative, and the for dative - in the spontaneous speech of seven children from the age of 1;6 to 5;0. The main findings were as follows. First, the double-ob ... Full text Cite

The Development of Relative Clauses in Spontaneous Child Speech

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · January 1, 2001 This study examines the development of relative clauses in the speech of four English-speaking children between 1;9 and 5;2 years of age. It is shown that the earliest relative clauses occur in presentational constructions that express a single proposition ... Full text Cite

First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · January 1, 2001 Usage-based models of language focus on the specific communicative events in which people learn and use language. In these models, the psycholinguistic units with which individuals operate are determined not by theoretical fiat but by observation of actual ... Full text Cite

Cultural Transmission:A View from Chimpanzees and Human Infants

Journal Article Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology · January 1, 2001 Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways that other primates are not, as evidenced most clearly by the fact that only human cultural traditions accumulate modifications over historical time (the ratchet effect). The key adaptation is one t ... Full text Cite

The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A corpus-based analysis

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · January 1, 2001 This article examines the development of finite complement clauses in the speech of seven English-speaking children aged 1;2 to 5;2.It shows that in most of children's complex utterances that seem to include a finite complement clause, the main clause does ... Full text Cite

'Separating the wheat from the chaff': A novel food processing technique in captive Gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla)

Journal Article Primates · January 1, 2001 Observation of a novel food processing technique is reported for captive zoo gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla). It is similar in function to that of Japanese macaques' wheat placer mining behaviour and consists of puffing/blowing air with the mouth onto a mixt ... Full text Cite

Could we please lose the mapping metaphor, please?

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 2001 Although Bloom gives more credit to social cognition (mind reading) than do most other theorists of word learning, he does not go far enough. He still relies fundamentally on a learning process of association (or mapping), neglecting the joint attentional ... Full text Cite

What preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical word orders

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 2001 Akhtar [J. Child Lang. 26 (1999) 339.] found that when 4-year-old English-speaking children hear novel verbs in transitive utterances with ungrammatical word orders (e.g., Elmo the tree meeked), they correct them to canonical SVO order almost all of the ti ... Full text Cite

Social and object support for early symbolic play

Journal Article Developmental Science · January 1, 2001 In this study we sought to determine the degree to which 2-to 3-year-old children use objects symbolically in the relative absence of adult symbolic actions or linguistic descriptions, and how the nature of objects influences symbolic play. Results reveale ... Full text Cite

Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2001 We conducted three experiments on social problem solving by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. In each experiment a subordinate and a dominant individual competed for food, which was placed in various ways on the subordinate's side of two opaque barriers. In so ... Full text Cite

The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2001 Primates follow the gaze direction of conspecifics to outside objects. We followed the ontogeny of this social-cognitive skill for two species: rhesus macaques and chimpanzees, in the first two experiments, using both a cross-sectional and a longitudinal d ... Full text Cite

Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use

Journal Article Animal Cognition · December 1, 2000 The results of three experiments are reported. In the main study, a human experimenter presented domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) with a variety of social cues intended to indicate the location of hidden food. The novel findings of this study were: (1) dog ... Full text Cite

Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects

Journal Article Animal Cognition · December 1, 2000 Chimpanzees follow conspecific and human gaze direction reliably in some situations, but very few chimpanzees reliably use gaze direction or other communicative signals to locate hidden food in the object-choice task. Three studies aimed at exploring facto ... Full text Cite

Factors affecting young children's use of pronouns as referring expressions.

Journal Article Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR · December 2000 Most studies of children's use of pronouns have focused either on the morphology of personal pronouns or on the anaphoric use of pronouns by older children. The current two studies investigated factors affecting children's choice of pronouns as referring e ... Full text Cite

Erratum: (Trends in Cognitive Sciences (April) 4:4 (156-163))

Journal Article Trends in Cognitive Sciences · May 1, 2000 Full text Cite

The item-based nature of children's early syntactic development

Journal Article Trends in Cognitive Sciences · April 1, 2000 Recent research using both naturalistic and experimental methods has found that the vast majority of young children's early language is organized around concrete, item-based linguistic schemas. From this beginning, children then construct more abstract and ... Full text Cite

Do young children have adult syntactic competence?

Journal Article Cognition · March 2000 Many developmental psycholinguists assume that young children have adult syntactic competence, this assumption being operationalized in the use of adult-like grammars to describe young children's language. This "continuity assumption" has never had strong ... Full text Cite

Culture and cognitive development

Journal Article Current Directions in Psychological Science · January 1, 2000 Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways that other primates are not. The difference can be clearly seen when the social learning skills of humans and their nearest primate relatives are systematically compared. The human adaptation for cu ... Full text Cite

Primate cognition: Introduction to the issue

Journal Article Cognitive Science · January 1, 2000 I introduce the special issue by: (1) outlining something of the relationship between mainstream cognitive science and the study of nonhuman primate cognition; (2) providing a brief overview of the scientific study of primate cognition and how the papers o ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 2000 We report a series of experiments on social problem solving in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. In each experiment a subordinate and a dominant individual were put into competition over two pieces of food. In all experiments dominants obtained virtually all o ... Full text Cite

Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds

Journal Article Infant Behavior and Development · December 1, 1999 The current study was a replication and extension of a study of infant imitative learning by Meltzoff (1995). Unlike the 18-month-old infants in that study (and other 18-month-olds in the current study), the 12-month-olds in this study did not frequently i ... Full text Cite

And what about the Chinese?

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · December 1, 1999 We discuss other recent studies on the acquisition of the German plural that do not support the dual-mechanism model. The attested overgeneralizations are not by default only, nor completely random, but predictable from subregularities based on the grammat ... Full text Cite

Do young children use objects as symbols?

Journal Article British Journal of Developmental Psychology · November 1, 1999 Much of young children's symbolic play is heavily scaffolded by adult symbolic action models, which children may imitate, and by adult verbal scripts. The current studies attempted to evaluate 18-35-month-old children's symbolic skills in the absence of su ... Full text Cite

Young children's overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs.

Journal Article Child development · November 1999 The present study examined English-speaking children's tendency to make argument structure overgeneralization errors (e.g., I disappeared it). Children were exposed to several English verbs of fixed transitivity (exclusively intransitive or exclusively tra ... Full text Cite

Causal understanding in primates in physical and psychological domain

Journal Article Sistemi Intelligenti · August 1, 1999 Full text Cite

A nonverbal false belief task: the performance of children and great apes.

Journal Article Child development · March 1999 A nonverbal task of false belief understanding was given to 4- and 5-year-old children (N = 28) and to two species of great ape: chimpanzees and orangutans (N = 7). The task was embedded in a series of finding games in which an adult (the hider) hid a rewa ... Full text Cite

Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · January 1999 Younger and older children (mean ages = 2 years 11 months and 3 years 5 months) learned 2 nonce verbs in a full passive or active transitive construction. When asked patient-focused questions encouraging passive-voice replies (e.g., "What happened to the b ... Full text Cite

How children constrain their argument structure constructions

Journal Article Language · January 1, 1999 We tested two hypotheses about how English-speaking children learn to avoid making argument structure errors such as Don't giggle me. The first is that children base their usage of verbs on membership in narrow-range semantic classes (Pinker 1989). The sec ... Full text Cite

The human adaptation for culture

Journal Article Annual Review of Anthropology · January 1, 1999 Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways that other primates are not, as evidenced most clearly by the fact that only human cultural traditions accumulate modifications over historical time (the ratchet effect). The key adaptation is one t ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 1999 Two experiments on chimpanzee gaze following are reported. In the first, chimpanzee subjects watched as a human experimenter looked around various types of barriers. The subjects looked around each of the barriers more when the human had done so than in a ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzee use of human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food

Journal Article Developmental Science · January 1, 1999 Two studies are reported in which chimpanzees attempted to use social cues to locate hidden food in one of two possible hiding places. In the first study four chimpanzees were exposed to a local enhancement cue (the informant approached and looked to the l ... Full text Cite

Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food

Journal Article Journal of Comparative Psychology · January 1, 1999 Ten domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different breeds and ages were exposed to 2 different social cues indicating the location of hidden food, each provided by both a human informant and a conspecific informant (for a total of 4 different social cues). ... Full text Cite

Learning to produce passive utterances through discourse

Journal Article First Language · December 1, 1998 Two studies of English-speaking children's acquisition of the passive construction are reported. In the first study children at 3.0 and 3.5 years of age were taught to produce full passive utterances with a nonce verb through rich discourse interaction. Al ... Full text Cite

Acquiring the transitive construction in English: the role of animacy and pronouns.

Journal Article Journal of child language · October 1998 Twenty-four children between 2;5 and 3;1 were taught two nonce verbs. Each verb was used multiple times by an adult experimenter to refer to a highly transitive action involving a mostly animate agent (including the child herself) and a patient of varying ... Full text Cite

Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach.

Journal Article The Behavioral and brain sciences · October 1998 To explain social learning without invoking the cognitively complex concept of imitation, many learning mechanisms have been proposed. Borrowing an idea used routinely in cognitive psychology, we argue that most of these alternatives can be subsumed under ... Full text Cite

Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (Homo sapiens).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · June 1998 This study investigates the understanding of others' intentions in 2- and 3-year-old children, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). During training, subjects learned to use a discriminative cue to select a baited box. During test ... Full text Cite

Primate causal understanding in the physical and psychological domains

Journal Article Behavioural Processes · February 1, 1998 Evidence for primates' understanding of causality is presented and discussed. Understanding causality requires the organism to understand not just that two events are associated with one another in space and time, but also that there is some 'mediating for ... Full text Cite

Reference: Intending that others jointly attend

Journal Article Pragmatics and Cognition · January 1, 1998 My approach to reference focuses on naturally occuring processes of communication, and in particular on children’s earliest referential activities. I begin by describing three different kinds of child gesture – ritualizations, deictics, and symbolic gestur ... Full text Cite

Young Children'S earliest transitive and intransitive constructions

Journal Article Cognitive Linguistics · January 1, 1998 Much of children's early syntactic development can be seen as the acquisition of sentence-level constructions that correspond to relatively complex events and states of affairs. The ctirrent study was an attempt to determine the relative concreteness (verb ... Full text Cite

Response to commentators

Journal Article Journal of Child Language · January 1, 1998 Full text Cite

Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age.

Journal Article Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development · January 1998 At around 1 year of age, human infants display a number of new behaviors that seem to indicate a newly emerging understanding of other persons as intentional beings whose attention to outside objects may be shared, followed into, and directed in various wa ... Full text Cite

Cooperative problem-solving and teaching in preschoolers

Journal Article Social Development · January 1, 1998 The current study investigated the ontogenetic origins of children's skills of cooperative problem-solving in a task involving two complementary roles. Participants were peer dyads of 24, 30, 36, and 42 months of age. Primary dyads were initially presented ... Full text Cite

Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions

Journal Article Infant Behavior and Development · January 1, 1998 This study explored infants' ability to discriminate between, and their tendency to reproduce, the accidental and intentional actions of others. Twenty 14- through 18-month-olds watched an adult perform a series of two-step actions on objects that made int ... Full text Cite

Uniquely primate, uniquely human

Journal Article Developmental Science · January 1, 1998 Two hypotheses about primate cognition are proposed. First, it is proposed that primates, but not other mammals, understand categories of relations among external entities. In the physical domain primates have special skills in tasks such as oddity, transi ... Full text Cite

Chimpanzee and human cultures

Journal Article Current Anthropology · January 1, 1998 Culture has traditionally been attributed only to human beings. Despite growing evidence of behavioral diversity in wild chimpanzee populations, most anthropologists and psychologists still deny culture to this animal species. We argue here that culture is ... Full text Cite

Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 1998 Individuals from five primate species were tested experimentally for their ability to follow the visual gaze of conspecifics to an outside object. Subjects were from captive social groups of chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, sooty mangabeys, Cercocebus atys to ... Full text Cite

Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children.

Journal Article Child development · December 1997 Forty-eight young children (2.5 and 3.0 years old) and 9 great apes (6 chimpanzees and 3 orangutans) participated in a hiding-finding game. An adult human experimenter (the Hider) hid a reward in 1 of 3 opaque containers aligned on a wooden plank. Another ... Full text Cite

Young children's productivity with word order and verb morphology.

Journal Article Developmental psychology · November 1997 Four studies examined English-speaking children's productivity with word order and verb morphology. Two- and 3-year-olds were taught novel transitive verbs with experimentally controlled argument structures. The younger children neither used nor comprehend ... Full text Cite

Differential productivity in young children's use of nouns and verbs.

Journal Article Journal of child language · June 1997 A fundamental question of child language acquisition is children's productivity with newly learned forms. The current study addressed this question experimentally with children just beginning to combine words. Ten children between 1;6 and 1;11 were taught ... Full text Cite

A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants;

Journal Article Human Development · January 1, 1997 The naturally occurring gestures of chimpanzees and prelinguistic human infants are compared. Considered as special cases are apes raised by humans as they gesture to humans, and children with autism. Overall, the most important differences between the ges ... Full text Cite

Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts.

Journal Article Journal of child language · February 1996 Previous studies have demonstrated that children aged 2;0 can learn new words in a variety of non-ostensive contexts. The current two studies were aimed at seeing if this was also true of children just beginning to learn words at 1;6. In the first study an ... Full text Cite

Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions

Journal Article British Journal of Developmental Psychology · January 1, 1996 Two studies of word learning in 24-month-old children are reported, one involving an object word (Study 1) and one involving an action word (Study 2). In both studies, non-verbal scripts of playing with novel objects/actions in particular ways were establi ... Full text Cite

The Role of Discourse Novelty in Early Word Learning

Journal Article Child Development · January 1, 1996 2 studies of word learning are reported. In Study 1, 24-month-old children and 2 adults played with 3 nameless objects. These objects were placed in a clear box along with a novel nameless object. The adults then displayed excitement about the contents of ... Full text Cite

The child's contribution to culture: A commentary on Toomela

Journal Article Culture and Psychology · January 1, 1996 Toomela (1996) has emphasized the psychological dimensions of the process by which human children become participants in cultures. I support his arguments with observations of chimpanzees, which are similar to humans in some ways but still do not live cult ... Full text Cite

Piagetian and Vygotskian Approaches to Language Acquisition

Journal Article Human Development · January 1, 1996 Both Piaget and Vygotsky were centrally concerned with the ontogenetic relationships between language, cognition, and social life. Recently, researchers have drawn on their observations and hypotheses to establish much closer links between these phenomena ... Full text Cite

Use of social information in the problem solving of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · September 1995 Fourteen juvenile and adult orangutans and 24 3- and 4-year-old children participated in 4 studies on imitative learning in a problem-solving situation. In all studies a simple to operate apparatus was used, but its internal mechanism was hidden from subje ... Full text Cite

Joint Attention and Imitative Learning in Children, Chimpanzees, and Enculturated Chimpanzees

Journal Article Social Development · January 1, 1995 In this study we compared the nature of the joint attentional interactions that occurred as chimpanzees and human children engaged with a human experimenter (E). Subjects were three chimpanzees raised mostly with conspecifics (mother‐reared), three chimpan ... Full text Cite

Commentary

Journal Article Human Development · January 1, 1995 Full text Cite

Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 1995 Previous studies have found that children can use social-pragmatic cues to determine "which one" of several objects or "which one' of several actions an adult intends to indicate with a novel word. The current studies attempted to determine whether childre ... Full text Cite

Do rats ape?

Journal Article Animal Behaviour · January 1, 1995 Full text Cite

Understanding the self as social agent

Chapter · January 1, 1995 This chapter discusses the early development of the understanding of self as social agent in the human species, and briefly discusses its ontogenetic and phylogenetic origins. The chapter outlines the canonical developmental sequence, focusing especially o ... Full text Cite

Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · December 1994 We report 3 studies of the referential pointing of 2 orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Chantek was raised in an enculturated environment; Puti, raised in a nursery, had a more typical captive life. In Experiment 1, flexibility of pointing behavior was investiga ... Full text Cite

The social learning of tool use by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus)

Journal Article Human Evolution · October 1, 1994 Very little is known about the social learning of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), especially in the context of problem-solving situations such as tool use. Sixteen orangutans were presented with a rake-like tool and desirable but out-of-reach food. Eight subj ... Full text Cite

THE INSTRUMENT IS GREAT, BUT MEASURING COMPREHENSION IS STILL A PROBLEM

Journal Article Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development · January 1, 1994 Full text Cite

The role of emotions in cultural learning

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1994 Full text Cite

The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study

Journal Article Primates · January 1, 1994 Observations of chimpanzee gestural communication are reported. The observations represent the third longitudinal time point of an ongoing study of the Yerkes Primate Center Field Station chimpanzee group. In contrast to observations at the first two time ... Full text Cite

Social cognition of monkeys and apes

Journal Article American Journal of Physical Anthropology · January 1, 1994 This paper reviews what is known about the social cognition of monkeys and great apes. The literature reviewed is divided into three main content areas: (1) social interaction, including knowledge of individuals, knowledge of social relationships, alliance ... Full text Cite

Learning Words in Nonostensive Contexts

Journal Article Developmental Psychology · January 1, 1994 Four word learning studies with 24-month-old children are reported. In Studies 1 and 2, an adult used a novel word to announce her intention to perform an action or to find an object. It was found that a knowledge of what action or object was impending-est ... Full text Cite

Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees.

Journal Article Child development · December 1993 In this study we compared the abilities of chimpanzees and human children to imitatively learn novel actions on objects. Of particular interest were possible differences between chimpanzees raised mostly with conspecifics (mother-reared) and chimpanzees ra ... Full text Cite

Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens).

Journal Article Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983) · June 1993 Common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and 2-year-old human children (Homo sapiens) were presented with a rakelike tool and a desirable but out-of-reach object. One group of subjects observed a human demonstrator use the tool in one way, and another group ob ... Full text Cite

It's imitation, not mimesis

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1993 Full text Cite

Twenty-three-month-old children have a grammatical category of noun

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 1993 This study investigated experimentally the nature and development of children's early productivity with nouns, both in verb-argument structure and with plural morphology. Eight 20- to 26-month-old boys and girls were, in the context of playing a game over ... Full text Cite

Twenty-five-month-old children do not have a grammatical category of verb

Journal Article Cognitive Development · January 1, 1993 This study investigated experimentally the nature and development of children's early productivity with verb-argument structure and verb morphology. Twenty-two to 25-month-old boys and girls were, in the context of playing a game over a several week period ... Full text Cite

Cultural learning

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1993 This target article presents a theory of human cultural learning. Cultural learning is identified with those instances of social learning in which intersubjectivity or perspective-taking plays a vital role, both in the original learning process and in the ... Full text Cite

Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts.

Journal Article Journal of child language · June 1992 Two studies of verb learning are reported. The focus of both studies was on children in their second year of life learning verbs in various pragmatic contexts. Of particular interest was the comparison of ostensive contexts--in which word and referent were ... Full text Cite

The social bases of language acquisition

Journal Article Social Development · January 1, 1992 A language is composed of conventional symbols shaped by their social‐communicative functions. Children acquire these symbols, both lexical and syntactic, in the context of culturally constituted event structures that make salient these functions. In the a ... Full text Cite

Author's response: On defining language: Replies to Shatz and Ninio

Journal Article Social Development · January 1, 1992 Full text Cite

Cognitive ethology comes of age

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1992 Full text Cite

The Effect of Video Context on Foreign Language Learning

Journal Article The Modern Language Journal · January 1, 1992 Full text Cite

Two-year-olds' conversations with their mothers and preschool-aged siblings

Journal Article First Language · January 1, 1992 The pragmatics of sibling-infant and mother-infant conversations were compared. Sixteen children, 22 to 26 months of age, were videotaped for 15 minutes in dyadic interaction with their mothers and for 15 minutes in dyadic interaction with their preschool- ... Full text Cite

Joint Attention and Conversation in Mother‐Infant‐Sibling Triads

Journal Article Child Development · January 1, 1991 The current study investigated the general nature of joint attentional and conversational interaction in mother‐infant‐sibling triads. 9 19‐month‐old infants and 9 24‐month‐old infants were videotaped during 20 min of free play with their mothers and presc ... Full text Cite

Objects are analogous to words, not phonemes or grammatical categories

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1991 Full text Cite

A Reply to Beck and Eubank

Journal Article Studies in Second Language Acquisition · January 1, 1991 Beck and Eubank (1991) criticize our recent SSLA article (Tomasello & Herron, 1989) on both theoretical and methodological grounds. While we appreciate their attempt to discuss and clarify important issues—and while they do make several sound and very inte ... Full text Cite

Young children's conversations with their mothers and fathers: differences in breakdown and repair.

Journal Article Journal of child language · February 1990 In this study we compared the conversations of mothers and fathers with their children at 1; 3 and 1; 9, with special attention to breakdown-repair sequences. We found that, overall, children and secondary caregiver fathers experienced more communicative b ... Full text Cite

Peer interaction in infant chimpanzees.

Journal Article Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology · January 1990 The peer interactions of 6 infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) ranging in age from 18 to 50 months were observed in a seminatural context. The infants and their mothers lived as members of a captive social group at the Yerkes Regional Primate Research Cen ... Full text Cite

Feedback for language transfer errors the garden path technique

Journal Article Studies in Second Language Acquisition · January 1, 1989 In this study we compared two methods for correcting language transfer errors in the foreign language classroom. Thirty-two English-speaking college students enrolled in two sections of an introductory French course served as subjects. Eight commonly encou ... Full text Cite

Data on language input: Incomprehensible omission indeed!

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1989 Full text Cite

Cognition as cause

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1989 Full text Cite

A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees

Journal Article Primates · January 1, 1989 A longitudinal study of chimpanzee gestural communication is reported. Subjects were seven 5- to 8-year-old members of a semi-natural group at the Yerkes Field Station. These were the same individuals observed by Tomasello et al. (1985) four years previous ... Full text Cite

Down the Garden Path: Inducing and correcting overgeneralization errors in the foreign language classroom

Journal Article Applied Psycholinguistics · January 1, 1988 In this study we compared two methods for teaching grammatical exceptions in the foreign language classroom. Thirty-nine students in two sections of an introductory college French course served as subjects. Eight target structures, exemplifying –exceptions ... Full text Cite

Well-fed organisms still need feedback

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1988 Full text Cite

The role of joint attentional processes in early language development

Journal Article Language Sciences · January 1, 1988 In this paper I examine the role of joint attentional processes in the child's early lexical acquisition and conversational interaction. In both cases I conclude that relatively extended periods of adult-child joint attentional focus on nonlinguistic entit ... Full text Cite

Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees

Journal Article Human Evolution · April 1, 1987 In the current study two groups of young chimpanzees (4-6 and 8-9 years old) were given a T-bar and a food item that could only be reached by using the T-bar. Experimental subjects were given the opportunity to observe an adult using the stick as a tool to ... Full text Cite

Learning to use prepositions: a case study.

Journal Article Journal of child language · February 1987 Full text Cite

Why the left hand?

Journal Article Behavioral and Brain Sciences · January 1, 1987 Full text Cite

Joint attention and early language.

Journal Article Child development · December 1986 This paper reports 2 studies that explore the role of joint attentional processes in the child's acquisition of language. In the first study, 24 children were videotaped at 15 and 21 months of age in naturalistic interaction with their mothers. Episodes of ... Full text Cite

Object permanence and relational words: a lexical training study.

Journal Article Journal of child language · October 1986 Full text Cite

Transactive Discussions With Peers and Adults

Journal Article Developmental Psychology · September 1, 1986 Piaget hypothesized that peer and adult-child discussions of moral dilemmas are qualitatively different. He asserted that children are more likely to use reasoning when interacting with peers. To test this hypothesis, the present study compared the interac ... Full text Cite

Linguistic Environment of 1- to 2-Year-Old Twins

Journal Article Developmental Psychology · March 1, 1986 The current study investigated differences in the language learning environments of singletons and twins, with special reference to pragmatic factors that might be expected to differ in dyadic and triadic interactive situations. Six twin pairs and 12 singl ... Full text Cite

Young children's responses to neutral and specific contingent queries.

Journal Article Journal of child language · February 1986 Full text Cite

Evidence for social referencing in young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).

Journal Article Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology · January 1986 A captive chimpanzee group was observed in order to determine the extent to which the social interactions of the infants and juveniles (18-50 months) were affected by their mothers' relationships with other adult group members. It was found that the young ... Full text Cite

The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees

Journal Article Journal of Human Evolution · January 1, 1985 Plooij (Action, Gesture and Symbol, Academic Press 1978; Before Speech, C.U.P. 1979) described some intentionally-produced communicatory gestures used by one-year-old chimpanzees on the Gombe Stream Reserve. The current study investigated the use of this t ... Full text Cite

Children's speech revisions for a familiar and an unfamiliar adult.

Journal Article Journal of speech and hearing research · September 1984 Initial characterizations of the communicative abilities of preschoolers stressed their egocentric nature. Recently, however, even 2-year-olds have been observed to adjust their speech appropriately in situations in which the listener provides feedback by ... Full text Cite

Young children's coordination of gestural and linguistic reference

Journal Article First Language · January 1, 1984 The current study investigated the relationship between young children's linguistic and nonlinguistic communicative strategies. Twenty-three children, 20-44 months of age, served as subjects. In a naturalistic setting, an adult gave signs of noncomprehensi ... Full text Cite

The effect of variation in sentence length on young children's attention and comprehension

Journal Article First Language · January 1, 1984 The effect of sentence length on children's attention and com prehension was studied. Twenty-five two- to five-year-olds were placed into three groups, High, Middle, and Low, based on their mean length of utterance (MLU). Subsequently, each child watched t ... Full text Cite

Joint attention and lexical acquisition style

Journal Article First Language · January 1, 1983 Recent research has documented systematic individual differences in early lexical development. The current study investigated the relation ship of these differences to differences in the way mothers and children regulate each other's attentional states. Mo ... Full text Cite